Forest Manners Exchange: Forest as a Place to Remedy Risky Behaviour of Adolescents: Mixed Methods Approach
International Journal of
Environmental Research
and Public Health
Article
Forest Manners Exchange: Forest as a Place to Remedy Risky
Behaviour of Adolescents: Mixed Methods Approach
Karolina Machácˇková 1,* , Roman Dudík 1 , Jirˇí Zelený 2 , Dana Kolárˇová 3, Zbyneˇk Vinš 2 and Marcel Riedl 1
1 Department of Forestry and Wood Economics, Czech University of Life Sciences Prague, Faculty of Forestry
and Wood Sciences, Kamýcká 129, 6-Suchdol, 16500 Praha, Czech Republic; dudik@fld.czu.cz (R.D.);
riedl@fld.czu.cz (M.R.)
2 Department of Hotel Management, Institute of Hospitality Management in Prague, Svídnická 506,
18200 Prague, Czech Republic; zeleny@vsh.cz (J.Z.); vins@vsh.cz (Z.V.)
3 Department of Languages, Institute of Hospitality Management in Prague, 18200 Prague, Czech Republic;
kolarova@vsh.cz
* Correspondence: machackovak@fld.czu.cz
Citation: Machácˇková, K.; Dudík, R.;
Zelený, J.; Kolárˇová, D.; Vinš, Z.;
Riedl, M. Forest Manners Exchange:
Forest as a Place to Remedy Risky
Behaviour of Adolescents: Mixed
Methods Approach. Int. J. Environ.
Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 5725.
https://doi.org/10.3390/
ijerph18115725
Abstract: This paper evaluates the impact of the forest environment on aggressive manifestations in
adolescents. A remedial educative programme was performed with 68 teenagers from institutions
with substitute social care with diagnoses F 30.0 (affective disorders) and F 91.0 (family-related
behavioural disorders), aged 12–16 years. Adolescents observed patterns of prosocial behaviour
in forest animals (wolves, wild boars, deer, bees, ants, squirrels and birds), based on the fact that
processes and interactions in nature are analogous to proceedings and bonds in human society. The
methodology is based on qualitative and quantitative research. Projective tests (Rorschach Test, Hand
Test, Thematic Apperception Test) were used as a diagnostic tool for aggressive manifestations before
and after forest therapies based on Shinrin-yoku, wilderness therapy, observational learning and
forest pedagogy. Probands underwent 16 therapies lasting for two hours each. The experimental
intervention has a statistically significant effect on the decreased final values relating to psychopathol-
ogy, irritability, restlessness, emotional instability, egocentrism, relativity, and negativism. Forest
animals demonstrated to these adolescents ways of communication, cooperation, adaptability, and
care for others, i.e., characteristics without which no community can work.
Keywords: forest fauna community; communication; social behaviour; aggression; projective tests;
Shinrin-yoku; forest pedagogy
Academic Editor: Takahide Kagawa
Received: 9 April 2021
Accepted: 24 May 2021
Published: 26 May 2021
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral
with regard to jurisdictional claims in
published maps and institutional affil-
iations.
Copyright: © 2021 by the authors.
Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland.
This article is an open access article
distributed under the terms and
conditions of the Creative Commons
Attribution (CC BY) license (https://
creativecommons.org/licenses/by/
4.0/).
1. Introduction
Every living organism manifests itself in relationship with other organisms in the
environment. However, sometimes relationships are disrupted by aggression and social
maladaptation, which is more serious when this concerns children and adolescents [1–3].
Every individual has aggression of varying intensity and to a different extent, and it
cannot be entirely ruled out of our lives. At the same time, aggression is an adaptation
mechanism that helps us survive and overcome various obstacles. [4,5]. The difference
between the concepts of aggression and aggressiveness can be summarised in two areas: (1)
aggressiveness is a specific trait, a character trait. It is determined biologically (heredity),
cognitively (learning) and psycho-socially (emotional, along with the influence of the
external environment) [6,7]. This property is within each person to a greater or lesser
extent [8]. (2) Aggression is understood as any form of behaviour intended to harm
someone intentionally [9,10] Aggression can be suppressed (without external expression),
verbal (swearing, writing complaints), against things (destruction, tearing, breaking objects
and things) and against animals and people [11–13]. According to the World Health Report
(WHO 2010) [14], in that period, 1.6 million people lost their lives in the world due to
violent behaviour. Half committed suicide, a third were murdered, and a fifth were lives
lost in armed conflict.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 5725. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18115725
https://www.mdpi.com/journal/ijerph
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 5725
2 of 20
Aggressive tendencies are created in humans in the first years of life based on heredi-
tary dispositions, instinctive equipment, upbringing, learning, previous experience, and
environmental influence [15]. Aggression must be controlled, cultivated, regulated and
directed in a pro-socially effective direction so that relations between people and society’s
norms are not disrupted [16].
Many young people cannot integrate properly into society, often accompanied by con-
flicts and personality problems, including increased aggressive behaviour [17]. Prevention
of undesirable behaviour in children and adolescents is one of the necessities for achieving
a certain quality of life in adulthood. In modern society, Kagan [18] identifies adolescence
as the riskiest period on the path to adulthood, and it is a sensitive period for the devel-
opment of risky and problematic behaviour. At the forefront of the interest in preventive
and corrective activities, there is the syndrome of risky and problem behaviour, which
includes truancy, bullying, extreme manifestations of aggression, racism and xenophobia,
the negative effect of sects, sexually risky behaviour, and addictive behaviour [19,20].
Adolescents try to cope with the developmental tasks of the transition from childhood
to adulthood through risky behaviour. Therefore, it is necessary to help them find healthy
alternatives, such as remedial educative programmes, to fulfil the same function [20–22].
The family, peers, and the immediate surroundings of the adolescent have a significant
protective role. Schools and other educational facilities have a special status, operating
across the board in an interdisciplinary, long-term, and continuous manner [23].
1.1. Youth Behavioural Strategy
In this research, attention was focused on the manifestations of aggressive behaviour
in adolescents. The standard procedure for managing aggressive manifestations is to limit
external irritant stimuli, to provide space for the patient to express his/her feelings, or
pharmacological intervention. The recommended methods to manage aggression include
creative activity, physical activity and relaxation exercises. Aggression can also be mastered
by direct experience or observation of other people’s social interactions, i.e., observational
learning [24–26]. The learning model could be family members, people from the immediate
circle, symbolic models from the mass media and virtual reality, or computer games with
violent and brutal themes.
An emerging treatment that utilises wilderness therapy to help adolescents struggling
with behavioural and emotional problems is considered here, i.e., outdoor behavioural
healthcare (OBH) [27]. Wilderness treatment is one option of care that effectively treats chil-
dren and adolescents presenting aggressive behaviour [28]. Wilderness therapy combines
traditional therapy techniques with group therapy in a wilderness setting, approached
with therapeutic intent [29]. Adolescents demonstrated marked improvements in the
following areas: anxiety and depression, substance abuse and dependency, disruptive
behaviour, defiance and opposition, impulsivity, suicidality, violence, sleep disruption,
school performance, and interpersonal relationships. Russel [29] conducted a study last-
ing for 45 days. Adolescent client well-being was evaluated using the Youth Outcome
Questionnaire (Y-OQ) and the Self Report-Youth Outcome Questionnaire (SR Y-OQ) [30].
Complete data sets at admission and discharge were collected for 523 client self-reports and
372 parent assessments. Results indicated that, at admission, clients exhibited symptoms
similar to inpatient samples, which were, on average, significantly reduced at discharge.
Outcomes had been maintained at 12-months post-treatment. [30,31]. Russel [31] evaluated
youth well-being 24-months after the conclusion of outdoor behavioural healthcare (OBH)
treatment and explored youth transition to various post-treatment settings. The results
suggest that 80% of parents and 95% of youths perceived OBH treatment as practical, the
majority of clients were doing well at school, and family communication had improved.
Aftercare was utilised by 85% of the youths and was perceived as a crucial component
in facilitating the transition from an intensive wilderness experience to family, peer and
school environments.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 5725
3 of 20
Other studies also show that participants who had walked in nature reported less
anger and more positive emotions than those who engaged in other activities (walking in
an urban area, sitting quietly while reading magazines or listening to music) [32–35]. The
time spent in nature improves physiological relaxation and the immune function recovery
response [36,37]. Nature therapy can provide emotional healing, decrease blood pressure,
improve a person’s general sleep-wake cycle, improve relationship skills, reduce stress,
and reduce aggression [38,39]. Research studies included in the Stanford analysis [40] have
also found that the natural environment develops potential, and participants gain both
tacit knowledge and necessary depth of knowledge.
1.2. Study Design Based on Forest Programme
Prosocial behaviour is a successful strategy used by human and non-human individu-
als living in stable, long-lasting social groups [41,42]. The human’s limbic system allows
us to experience joy, sadness, fear, and pleasure; these brain structures are common to
humans, mammals, birds, and fish at a significantly lower evolutionary stage [43]. This
finding is followed by several studies dealing with animal emotions [44–46]. The authors
concluded that animals show affection, care for their species, can mourn and face danger
together. Another point of view is provided by Pavlík and Kopcˇaj [47–49], claiming that
processes and interactions in nature are analogous to human social interactions. Rösler [50]
introduces the term “Nature Ideas Exchange”: nature offers many analogies applicable to
practice, via which the principles of prosocial, positive emotional behaviour and coopera-
tion can be presented uniquely, as caring for others, coexistence, cooperation, adaptation,
and innovation occur every day in nature. The constant pressure towards assimilation
has led to high-level specialisations, sophisticated survival strategies, and collaborative
models [50].
We, therefore, assumed that this could work the other way around: if young people
with risky behaviour could not acquire prosocial behaviour patterns such as care, fidelity,
compassion, cooperation and adaptation in their families, this tacit knowledge could be
gained by observation in nature of forest fauna examples. In the forest environment, the
effects of therapy on reducing aggression can be combined with observational learning.
1.3. The Aim of the Research
Eisikovits [51] states that residential education should include the best possible educa-
tion methods, re-education, and psychotherapy. Without these, alternative institutional
care would merely consist of isolation of the child from society. Therefore, an unorthodox
remedial educative programme was performed for adolescents from institutions offer-
ing substitute social care, suffering mental problems with severe risky behaviour (abuse,
criminality, truancy, aggression, early sex life). The established diagnoses of adolescents
are given in chapter 2.1. The research aimed to verify the transformation in adolescents’
attitudes and behaviour based on forest therapy focused on forest animals’ emotional
lives. This research aimed to identify whether probands’ teamwork and social adaptation
would increase. The following question has guided the study: is it possible to reduce
aggressive behaviour with forest therapies based on observing the social behaviour of
forest animals with the simultaneous therapeutic action of Shinrin-yoku and Outdoor
Behavioural Therapy (OBH)?
The following hypothesis was established: Experimental intervention in the form of
forest therapy in groups “A” and “B” will reduce manifestations of aggression compared
to the original values measured before forest therapy.
2. Materials and Methods
Although the idea of OBH and wilderness therapies is remarkable and the study
by Russell [29–31] yields positive results, he admits that OBH and wilderness therapy’s
effectiveness reveals a consistent lack of theoretical basis, methodological shortcomings and
problematic results difficult to replicate [31]. Therefore, we have focused on the primary
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 5725
4 of 20
tool for influencing attitudes and behaviour: remedial educative programmes [52] based on
Shinrin-yoku, observational learning and forest pedagogy methods. Standardised psycho-
diagnostic instruments (projective tests) were used to evaluate adolescents’ aggressiveness
before and after taking part in the remedial educative programme.
2.1. Forest Therapy and Shinrin-Yoku
Forest therapy is a scientifically based method, the results of which are confirmed
by independent research. Its foundations are based on the Japanese Shinrin-yoku tech-
nique [53,54]. It has been reported that forest environments have beneficial effects on
human health: increased natural killer (N.K.) cell activity and in the number of N.K. cells;
increase in intracellular anti-cancer proteins; blood pressure lowering and decrease in
stress hormones (urinary adrenaline, noradrenaline, salivary cortisol). Up-to-date studies
show that, after a few hours in the forest, the level of stress hormones falls sharply, and
the immune system’s activity increases. The parasympathetic nervous system is activated
due to phytoncides-chemicals released into the air mainly by conifers, auditory stimuli of
wild birds singing, and visual stimuli of sunlight shining through the leaves [55–61]. In the
Profile of Mood States (POMS) test, forest environments reduce anxiety, depression, anger,
fatigue, confusion and increase the score for vigour [53], and especially for anxiety [62].
It is a therapeutic method that can be used to prevent the treatment and rehabilitation of
stress disorders and civilisation diseases and help treat mental disorders such as anxiety-
depressive disorders [53]. Kotera and Fido [63,64] demonstrated that the mean scores for
mental well-being, self-compassion, common humanity, and mindfulness had increased
significantly from pre-retreat to post-retreat.
2.2. Forest Pedagogy
Forest pedagogy is forest-related environmental education [65] based on the experien-
tial method, using senses, i.e., based on experiences and feelings. According to Pestalozzi’s
concept of learning with head, heart, and hand, forest pedagogy’s basic principle is the
perception of nature by all senses [66]. Understanding ethical values emerge through the
perception of situations, nature, and other people. Forest pedagogy develops emotional
intelligence, supporting cooperation and teamwork, self-awareness and co-responsibility.
The programme is carried out in groups; the individual is part of the group and is con-
stantly exposed to many social stimuli. Forest pedagogy deals with methods such as
interview, brainstorming, brainwriting, discussion, demonstration, practical activities,
thematic games, competitions, simulation, situational methods and dramatisation, buzz
groups, incident resolution method, maze method, aquarium method, role-playing, stag-
ing, dramatisation, themed games, and competitions [65,67–69]. The following activities
were selected from the PAWS textbook for teenagers: name tag and symbol; animal rights;
eco-audit; desert island.
2.3. Projective Tests as a Diagnostic Tool for Aggressive Manifestations
Projective tests are used to diagnose aggressive tendencies [6]. Doctors use these
tests to determine patients’ aggressive potential as part of a psychological examination.
Projective methods show implicit personality motives, and their results are challenging to
distort by intentional dissimulation. We focused on three projective methods, which have
been standardised as administration, evaluation, and interpretation: the Rorschach test,
the Hand test, and the Thematic Apperception Test, used to examine patients with risky
behaviour [70–72].
Projective techniques confront the tested person with an indefinite situation, to which
he/she will subjectively react according to the interpretation of its meaning. The ambiguity
of the situation can reveal latent personality components that the individual is unaware
of [73]. Projective tests show the examined person’s psychological presentation and relation
to the surrounding world by confronting the individual with a stimulating situation. Indi-
viduals perceive and interpret the tested material to reflect their psychological functioning
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 5725
5 of 20
because they project onto the situation their thinking processes, emotions, needs, anxieties,
and intrapsychic conflicts [6,73].
2.3.1. The Hand Test
The Hand test is a projective technique that should predict aggressive behaviour
and map interpersonal tendencies [70,74–76]. A hand symbol is an essential tool of social
contact and a means of controlling the material world; the hand plays a vital role in body
language and can be a source of pleasure and pain [72]. The Hand test consists of nine
cards on which the shape of a human hand is drawn, and the tenth card is empty. The
respondent should tell what emotion the picture expresses: “What can this hand do”?
Whether the answers express affection, communication, exhibition, acquisition, failure,
reciprocity, dependence, showing off, directive, aggression, activity, passivity, fear or
tension is assessed [77]. These categories are grouped into: (1) interpersonal responses
related to other people; (2) environmental responses related to the impersonal area and the
impersonal environment; (3) maladaptive responses, including formulations exhibiting
weakness, external inhibition, failure to meet needs; (4) answers expressing distance. In
this research, the Hand test was administered, evaluated, and interpreted according to
Lecˇbych [78].
2.3.2. The Rorschach Test (ROR)
The test consists of ten differently coloured boards: black and white, colourful (dark +
red), and coloured boards. The purpose of the test is to determine the projection of thought
processes and personality traits on indefinite objects, which have the form of ink stains
of various colours and shapes [72,79–83]. After presenting the board, the tested person is
asked the question, “What could it be, what is it like?” From the ROR answers, it is possible
to deduce several variables: emotion, aggressive tendencies, personality, interpersonal
relationships, the subject’s relationship to himself, and cognitive functions. The IQ band
can be approximately estimated from the test.
Aggression can be assessed in ROR according to (1) how the stain is interpreted in
terms of its integrity, (2) according to the determinant’s experience, (3) according to the
content of interpretation of humans, animals, and other objects and (4) according to the
occurrence of standard and original answers [84]. For example, the red parts of the card
are often perceived as blood; their reaction will indicate how the test taker manages the
feelings associated with anger or physical harm. The dark and shadowy parts of the card
can cause a situation in which the test taker feels depressed. Pure colour interpretations
are an indicator of affective and extroverted focus. Hue answers indicate the dominance
of the intellect over emotions and affective adaptability. Chiaroscuro responses indicate
moods (mostly dysphoric), suggesting a lack of tendency to control moods and an inability
to control dysphoric reactions. The interpretation of stain shows the intensity and the
importance of social bonds for the patient. Interpretations of animals are the most frequent
content category, and if this value increases, it indicates stereotypes, unproductivity of
thinking, and cumbersomeness or rigidity of thinking.
The answers were evaluated according to the frequency of banal (usual) and original
responses. Banal responses occur in 30% of healthy population protocols. They are
considered an indicator of intellectual contact and social adaptation of thinking. A higher
incidence of these responses may also indicate rigidity or anxious–depressive syndrome.
Administration and scoring in clinical practice were implemented according to Exner’s
Comprehensive System. Individual scores were evaluated based on a quantitative basis,
thus allowing statistical verification [85,86]. A structured procedure characterises the Exner
system for signing and interpreting results. It is a sophisticated system based on detailed
research, extending the perceptual-cognitive basis of the Rorschach method, which affects
diverse aspects of the responses (Exner and Harsa).
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 5725
6 of 20
2.3.3. The Thematic Apperception Test (TAT)
The TAT involves a series of picture cards depicting various ambiguous characters,
scenes, and situations. Respondents must come up with a story for each of them. TAT is
suitable for assessing or capturing the degree of interpersonal conflict and behavioural
disorder [87–89]. The test consists of 31 pictures (boards) on which people (women, men,
children) are drawn in many different life situations. One picture (number 16) consists of
only a white area. Here, the subject has the task of projecting an idea of his arbitrary image
and story: how the story goes and how it ends, what individuals think and feel, why the
situation occurred, and the story’s resolution.
It is assumed that the examined person identifies with the characters and interprets the
situation depending on his/her experience and conscious and unconscious needs [90–92].
2.4. Focus Groups
A focus group was interviewed to obtain the most valuable data from respondents
through their mutual interaction [93], using group dynamics. Interaction structures re-
flected the division of social status (superiority, equivalence, subordination) and relation-
ships between communicators (friendship, indifference, antagonism). The focus of the
discussion was on the self-regulation of behaviour, cognition of emotions, and attention.
The focus group sessions lasted 45 min, and 12 persons participated in each group. The
group members were acquainted with the rules (all interviews take place in the forum, all
members present can participate in the discussion, everyone has the right to express their
opinion, everyone has the right to refuse an answer). There was an effort made for only
one participant to speak in the group at a time, i.e., so that participants could take turns in
expressing their opinions. For critical statements, transcription was performed (including
substandard expressions).
The probands were asked the following questions:
1. Do you think that the social bonds of animals are transferable to the human world?
2. Which animal trait is the least transferable to the human world?
3. Does any animal community remind you of a similar bond in your family? (For
example, the dominant father, like the wolf, is the pack leader.)
4. Which animal community do you like the most and why?
5. In which animal community would you like to be a cub?
6. In which community do you see the most substantial internal ties? Where does a pack
member deploy their own life for the sake of their species? Is there a stronger bond
between a wolf and a wolf cub, a female wild boar and a piglet, or a roe deer and a
fawn?
7. Which animal community offers the best living conditions in packing status, access to
food, safety?
8. Which community is the smartest?
9. Which warning system do you consider the most perfect, and which is the most
altruistic?
The Focus Group aimed to discover attitudes towards the given issues and the most
critical aspects influencing the answers. The moderator (one of the researchers) encouraged
the probands to explain their points of view.
2.5. The Research Group
In the research group were adolescents from three institutions offering substitute
social care with severe behavioural disorders, for which institutional treatment was ordered.
Probands were selected by a specialist in psychiatry and a psychologist based on their
documentation and anamnestic data, from which it was realistic to assume aggressive
behaviour. The research group consisted of 68 probands (43 boys and 25 girls) 12–16 years
old (average age was 14.8 years) along with (1) adolescents from the diagnostic range F
30.0—mood disorders, affective disorders (n = 37); (2) adolescents from diagnostic range F
91.0—behavioural disorders related to family relationships (n = 31) [94]. The entry criterion
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 5725
7 of 20
was aggressive behaviour or tendency (manifest, latent, auto-aggressive, hetero-aggressive).
The research group also consisted of: (3) a control group of 1st-year high school students
with a medical focus, without diagnosed behavioural disorders, aged 15 years (n = 34). In
the control group, the presence of a mental disorder was the exclusion criterion. The control
group was chosen to obtain a homogeneous group and because it was simple to motivate
medical school students for a voluntary psychological examination. In all volunteers from
the control group, the attending physician required a psychological examination focusing
on aggression assessment. Members of the control group participated only in psychological
testing; they did not undergo forest therapy.
2.6. The Research Course, Data Collection and Research Instruments
We used the therapeutic effect of Shinrin-yoku therapy, which was supplemented
with forest pedagogy and observational learning focusing on principles of adaptation,
partnership, and cooperation in forest animals. Forest therapies took place twice a week
for two months (January–February 2020, two hours of therapy), and probands underwent
16 therapies lasting for two hours each in the forest, accompanied by a forest pedagogue
(forester, graduate of a certified course in forest pedagogy), warden, and one of the authors
of this paper. Adolescents were under the constant supervision of experienced mental
health professionals (wardens), who could immediately intervene if any aggressive out-
burst occurred. The maximum group size was eight people according to the internal
guidelines of the institution.
First, the forest pedagogue explained how individual animals in the forest behave
and their specifics. He used demonstration and showed strategic partnerships, coopera-
tion, adaptation, care, loyalty, courage and care using living bees, ants, deer, wild boars,
squirrels, wolves, and birds. Probands worked in teams, searching for parallels between
the forest ecosystem’s commonality and human society through observation, discussion,
brainstorming, mind maps, educational games, and experiences using forest pedagogy
methods [65,67–69].
Probands were examined with a battery of selected standardised diagnostic methods.
The test battery consisted of three standardised projective tests described above. The testing
was part of a comprehensive psychological examination, which also included personality
questionnaires and inventories. Testing was performed in the following order: The Hand
Test, 10 min, Rorschach Test (ROR), 20 min, and Thematic Apperception Test (TAT), 20 min.
For TAT, it is also permissible to select a certain number of boards (for example, 10) suitable
for a certain problem area, and these are exposed during the examination. We also chose
this variant and selected boards for which a higher incidence of aggressive tendencies or
responses was assumed. Selected boards were included: 3 BM, 7 BM, 8 BM, 14, 15, 16
(blank board), 17 GF, 18, BM, 18 GF, 20. The boards were chosen to reflect the patient’s
characteristics and feelings such as suicidality, aggression, depression, feelings about
death, fear, tension, and relationships with parental or important authorities. On the 16th
freeboard, the patient had to create the scene and describe the story in it.
Parametricity was detected by the Shapiro-Wilk test (α = 5%) in both groups for
all questions. The measured values indicated a “continuous variable” character, which
corresponds to the descriptive statistic. Where parametricity was detected, a T-test for
dependent samples was used (α = 5%), and where parametricity was not detected, the
Wilcoxon test (α = 5%) was used [95]. The state of the values before the experimental
intervention and after the experimental intervention were compared. Therefore, two tests
comparing dependent samples were used because an experimental intervention took place
in each examined case.
3. Results
3.1. Parallels found between Prosocial Behaviour of Forest Animals and Human Society
In the example of bee and plant, where bees collect pollen and guarantee the flower’s
reproduction, the probands understood the essence of mutually beneficial cooperation.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 5725
8 of 20
Probands discovered the interconnected and functional system of teamwork and care
for others, in which everyone has a role and place and is willing to accept a part in the
community of beehive and anthill, where the workload of each member of the community
is given (some are responsible for protection and safety; others care for larvae; others
forage). Based on the wolf pack and deer community (Canis lupus, Cervidae), probands
understood that those who can learn from the older generation could expect longer life;
and how teamwork and solidarity work in real-time: the pack or tribe must be collaborative
and pull together. Conformity, tolerance and the necessity to adapt to the environment
was conceived via the swarm and meadow grasshopper (Conocephalus discolour): a flock
of animals behaves and moves like one living organism with a certain logic, without
anyone commanding them. Insects blend in colour with their surroundings or mimic
the appearance of wasps, bees, and bumblebees. Probands detected the principle of
beneficial exchange based on ants (Lasius niger), termites (Isoptera) and aphids (Aphidoidea).
A warning signal of the common jay (Garrulus glandarius) and great tit (Parus major) to
which other animals responded by hiding in safety was understood as worrying about
others. Adolescents summarised that altruism increases in value when we must consciously
and actively deny something to help someone else, e.g., the great tit, who warned others,
found herself in danger because she drew attention to the predator. The fact that energy
invested into social cohesion creates remarkably resilient communities to external threats
was perceived in the examples of communities of wild boars (Sus scrofa). After detecting
that that common squirrel (Sciurus vulgaris) takes foreign orphaned cubs into their care,
adolescents stated that there are more variations of maternal love, and whether maternal
feelings arise from a subconscious stimulus or originate in conscious thinking is not critical
to its quality. Probands discovered partnerships based on exchange, liaison and tolerance
between wolves (Canis lupus) and ravens (Corvus frugilegus). They realised that there is
nothing wrong with disparate relationships between predator and blackbirds.
Examples from forest fauna were intentionally described simply, adapted to the age
and mental skills of the probands.
3.2. Evaluation of the Rorschach Test
The findings presented in this section result from psycho-diagnostic evaluations in 68
probands and a control group, examined by a battery of three projective tests before and
after forest therapy. These results emerge from individual measurements for individual
tests.
Group A includes probands with diagnosis F 30.0. affective disorders, 37 persons.
Group B includes probands with diagnosis F 91.0. family-related behavioural dis-
orders, 31 persons. Table 1 presents group averages of ROR test before and after forest
therapy.
Table 1. Group averages of ROR test before and after forest therapy.
ROR
Control
Group
Group A 1
Before
Therapy
After
Therapy
Statistical
Test Results
Group B 1
Before
Therapy
After
Therapy
Statistical
Test Results
Pure colour
answers
0.41
0.83 ± 0.06
0.79 ± 0.05
3 p < 0.01 *
z = 4.62
0.59 ± 0.13
0.51 ± 0.10
2 p < 0.01 *
t = 4.75
Interpretation
of animals
4.99
6.26 ± 0.77
5.87 ± 0.64
3 p < 0.01 *
z = 4.27
6.87 ± 1.14
5.91 ± 0.83
3 p < 0.01 *
z = 4.80
Interpretation
of blood
0.04
0.19 ± 0.05
0.11 ± 0.04
2 p < 0.01 *
t = 8.44
0.13 ± 0.06
0.08 ± 0.02
3 p < 0.01 *
z = 4.11
Contact with
reality
4.72
5.09 ± 0.88
5.01 ± 0.89
2 p = 0.01 *
t = 2.19
4.72 ± 0.79
3.99 ± 0.87
3 p < 0.01 *
z = 3.62
1 Values represent mean values ± standard deviations. 2 T-test for dependent samples (α = 5%). 3 Wilcoxon matched pairs test (α = 5%).
Both tests were used to detect statistically significant differences before and after the experimental intervention. The * character means a
statistically significant difference.
t = 2.19
z = 3.62
1 Values represent mean values ± standard deviations. 2 T-test for dependent samples (α = 5%).3 Wilcoxon matched pairs
test (α = 5%). Both tests were used to detect statistically significant differences before and after the experimental interven-
tion. The * character means a statistically significant difference.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 5725
9 of 20
The results show that the experimental intervention has a statistically significan
fect (p < 0.01) on the achieved final values without exception. The experimental inter
tionTrheedruecseusltsvsahluowesthinatbthoethexgpreoruimpesnAta,l Bin,taernvdenatliol nfohuars taessttasti(sctioclaolluyrssig, nainicmanatlesf,fbeclot od, rea
(p < 0.T01h)eorne tihseaanchoiteivcedablneadl veaclrueeasswe itnhobuotthextchepetfioirns.tTahnedexsepceorinmdenptaliri,natesrvshenotwion in Figu
rIendugcreosuvpaluBe,sfionrbeostththgreoruappsieAs, Bre, danudceadll fvoualruteesstsp(rceovloiuorus,salyniampaplse, abrloinodg, irneatlihtye).upper qua
andTinhecrreeiassaednoltoicweaebrleqdueacrrteialesevinalbuoeths t(hpe<fir0s.t0a1n; dt s=ec4o.7n5d)p. aTihr,iassfsahcotwisn rinefFliegcutreed1.by a sig
IicnnacgnrretoalyusepldoBlw,ofwoerreeersdtqtuhsaterartanilpdeieavsrardleudedusec(vepdi<avt0ia.ol0un1e;sftop=rre4gv.7iro5o)uu.sTplyhBaisp.fpaFecoatrriisntrgheeiflneAtchteegdurobpyupepar,sqitguhnaerifitiocleuanatlntildeyr value
leolwimeriendastteadndbayrdthdeevtriaetaiotnmfeonr tgr(opu<p0B.0. F1o; rzt=he4A.62g)r.oFurpo, mthetohuetlfiierrsvt arlouuenwdasoeflRimOinRatteedsting res
bityisthceletareratthmaetnatd(opl<es0c.e0n1;tszw=it4h.6d2)i.agFnromsedthme fiorosdt rdoiusnodrdoefrsROanRdteasftfiencgtirveesudlits,oirtders had
ims oclsetarsitghnatifiacdaonletsdceinfftiscuwlittyh mdiagnnaogsiendgmimoopduldsiisvoerdteernsdaenndciaeffse.cItnivethdeiisroradnesrws hearsd, purely
tohuermedosatnsisgwniercsawntedrieffirceuplteyatmedan, argeipnrgeismenptuilnsgiveimtepnudlesnicviietsy. ,Iwn thhiecihr awnsaws ehrasr, dpuerretloy control
cacapoondomldoanpupaeordntenattdeponptatsrntopoesrcdwseioodaecmrolisamnilwnoinneraoarmtrteeemssrs.e,,spi.Ti..eeThe.ah,.it,tsiehstdiehin,netrdedentiipeccdraaneettndesecesesynrntretcioednydudgucitiseomccdehdpdaruiarsgtrlcsieaoihvteniafioatfryelng,ccaetwoslnehactfnirofcodenhlc,utwtrwnsoabhlasr,eanhrwkedaerhtddhueeeenrmreibntoortsthaticiokenonenciadnttlirivsotetyelim.ncottiivoenac
TThheeuunnbablaalnacnedceadffeacftfievcetcivome cpoomnepntodnoemntindaotemd,inwahtiechd,mwaihnilcyhshmowaisntlhyesuhnocwonstrtohlelabulnecontrol
aannxxieietytyefefeffcet.ct.
FFiigguurere1.1R. ORROfRorfpourrpe ucorleoucroalonuswr earns.sNwoetress.fNoroGtreaspfho1rG6:rBaTpbhe1for6e:trBeTatmbeefnot;rAeTtraefatetrmtreenattm; AenTt; after tre
GmAenGtr;oGupAAG; GroBuGproAu;pGBB; CGOropuurpe Bco;lCouOr apnuswreercso;lAoNurinatnesrpwreetrast;ioAnNofianntiemrpalr;eBtLatiinotenrporfeatantiiomnal; BL in
opfrbeltoaotdio; nREofcobnltoaoctdw; RithErceoalnittya.ct with reality.
In probands with a family-related behavioural disorder, animal interpretation re-
sponsIens apprpoebaarenddms owstitfhreqauefanmtlyi,lays-rsehloawtendinbFeihgauvreio2u. rIfatlhde iasnoirmdaelrr,esapnoinmseasl eixncteeerdpretation
tshpeonnosrems(a30p%p)e,athreeydamreoosfttefnreasqsuoecinattelyd,waisthsthhoewdenprinessFivigeusyrend2r.oImf ethreelaatendimtoatlhirneksipnognses ex
itnheenxiobrilmity(, 3st0e%re)o,ttyhpeeys, aorremoofnteontoansysoofcaiassteodciawtioitnhptrhoecedsesepsr[e6s]s. iTvheesreynisdaronmotieceraeblaleted to th
decrease in both the first and second pair. In the second pair, the positive effect is even
higher because the value decrease is higher than in the first pair. There is also a significant
reduction in the group’s standard deviation in the second pair that became more constant
(p < 0.01; z = 4.80). For group A, an outlier previously appearing as a very high value was
reduced by the experimental intervention. Instead, a new outlier appeared, representing
the low value of evaluation (p < 0.01; t = 4.27).
hbilgehdeercbreeacsaeuisne bthoethvtahlueefidrsetcarenadseseicsohnigdhpearitrh. Ianntihnetsheecfoinrsdt pair., Tthheepreoissitaivlseoeaffseicgtniisfi
rheidghuecrtiboencianutsheethgerovuaplu’sesdtaencrdeaarsde disehvigathieorntihnatnhienstehceofnidrspt paiarirth. Tathbereecaismaelsmo oarseigcnonifsi
(rped<u0c.t0i1o;nzin= 4th.8e0g).rFoourpg’srostuapndAa,radndoeuvtilaiteiropnrienvtihoeusleycoanpdpepaariirntghast baevcearmyehmigohrveaclounes
r(ped<u0c.e0d1;bzy=t4h.e80e)x.pFeorrimgreonutapl Ain,taenrvoeunttliioenr.pIrnesvteioauds,lay napewpeoauritnligerasapapveearryedh,igrhepvraelsueen
Int.
J.
Environ.
Res.
Public
Health
2021,tr1he8de, 5u7lo2c5wedvbayluteheofeexvpaelruimatieonnta(lpi<nt0e.r0v1e;ntt=io4n.2. 7In).stead,
a
new
outlier
10 of 20
appeared, represen
the low value of evaluation (p < 0.01; t = 4.27).
Figure 2. ROR for animal interpretation.
FFiigguurree2.2R. ROOR Rforfoanr iamnailminatleripnrteetraptiroent.ation.
Another item tested was blood interpretation, as demonstrated in Figure 3.
iacssncphpvuoiootcnenlAvtsAdseineeninsnossgtstorhiibetnnoeslhorsvnve,oio,oatrdeallnvvmicidntiiuenntatetemnggersxptdbiebretdelliteoostywsa.toottrVaieddeosadsnrbiisisnnol,woattuaoeersanedrrsipdporirnfrrbitaeetteelanttnortaaxipoanttiriisdegeoosttosnnayitctsnis.iimoaVtaatenuerrar,ldeeirpaifswrooreodffuimtttehaseemtnntaiihornoenraaniesssstxt,ussarcoorataeritccssoneiisudaagdonttifeesndtdidminFinmitgwwogrsauuniipqrtltsesuihhtyrif3ccraa.khotnnRliemycedeesatpxexctnihtoccnisenevisssaosFestneusi,oogrffuroirinnuettnrr3daa.
aqcghugiicrcektselsynivsaeicoitmniv,pautclesueatseg[9gd6rise1tsr0se2is]v.seT, ahimnerdpe uiaslnsaxeniseot[ty9ic6.eVa1ba0lre2iod].ueTcsrheiraersrieetaiintsibnaogtnhsotttihmieceuarlbistlferaondmdescterhceoeansdeurinrobuontdh
pfqiaurisritc(kpaln<yd0a.s0ce1tic;vota=ntde8.a4p4ga)g.irTeh(spesid<vee0c.ri0em1as;petui=nlsv8ea.s4lu[49e)6.Tu1hc0teu2]ad.teiTocnhres ariseseivsiindaevnnatolitunicebeofaltbuhlcegtrudoauetpciorse,nabssueitsinevbidoethn
ebfivorestnht magnordoreuscpeoscn,osbtnaudnttlpeyvaiienrng(rmpou<opr0eB.0c(1po;n<ts0t=.a0n81;.t4lzy4=)i.n4T.1gh1r)eo. udpecBre(aps<e 0in.01v;azlu=e4f.l1u1c)t.uations is evide
both groups, but even more constantly in group B (p < 0.01; z = 4.11).
FFiigguurree3.3G. Grarfa3fR3ORROfoRr ftoher itnhteripnrteetarptiorentoaftibolonoodf. blood.
sFiiggnuiTfirTehchae3ne.ltGaldsartaisfftifte3eirtmeRenmOcteeRstwetfeosdatrsetwdhobaewssienacrtosvenerctpdoarcnientttaawgtciriotothnuwproieftBahbli(lrtopyeo,<adal.0ict.cy0o1,r;dazicnc=go3rtd.o6i2Fn)i,ggbuturote Fn4io.gtuSintraegtir4sot.iucSpatlalAytistically
(npif=ic0a.T0n1ht;edt l=iaff2se.t1ri9et)neimcnetwtheesactseoodnbtrwsoelarosvfecrdoeanilntiatygc,trwowhuiipcthhBirs(epaan<liet0ys.s0,e1an;ctzciao=lrm3d.ie6nd2gi)a,ttbooruFbtiengtowutreeienn4g.nreSoetudatspisAtic(apl=ly
atnn=ifd2icm.a1on9t)tividneisftfhienertechnoecnseutrwbojelacost’fosrbiensateelrirtvayce,tdiwoinhnsigcwhriotihus tpahneBee(xspstee<rnn0ta.i0la1eln;mzvie=rdo3ni.am6t2oe)nr,tb. euTthtweneroaetnnignnegeoerfodthusepanAd(mp =o
ritne=alt2he.s1et9as)teuinibnjtdehecext’cissoinnnttrehorelarocaftnirgoeenaoslfitw1yi,8twhpohtihinctehs.eiTsxhtaeenrnneoasrmsleeonnftvitahileromrenaemldeeisanttatot.erTibnhedetewxraiesne5nge7npeooefidntshtsae. nrdeaml oe
Winedatekhxeeniisnugibn(jsetihcgtenisracinantgetelryaobcfeti1loown8s4pwpoointinhttss.t)hTieshcehxantroearcnmtearliosetfincthvoefirmroenoarmle eseesnvttae.treeThmineednretaaxnl idgsies5oordf7etprhsoe.inrtesa.lWe
WoreWeienfnntedefaiiaannraekndggkxntanaeti((sissesosyssiin,ggcsni,arnnicnrt.iieaahffb.tnii,eicceofaaorbnfannroeanutttlhlilfngtyyooydeu,sbbeoionneerwfdllpao1ohusiwwonyt8ic“sphdp4t4iosocoysppinetcnhoosoh,toiitsnonsus.estthgvTsseah))eshrtre,sieie.sstphneAccsevonhyhewrcaaimrhnorreocaralorppccdefattsaeewtytshrrheciiiitseeshhststioiri,goccepcntaohaioolgetffhrncesaisimm”tentiavsbtool,tueyrrecteedaohiebngsasfiodeevncvvveeiietteexstih7rrvoieeeespriromm5mdwineeee7otnfnnsirtpclttaidiaasoltllsinddotiisrss.oomWrredd
arWelsteoaarpkdanathetoisolsongc,aiicn.ael..b, Iefnocfroretuahnsoedsdeicnwonphtsrooylcodhforseneaosli,ttysehmvaaeryre,eftohpreseywxcahomroplpdlaew,thbieitehdsu,oecthotoegrnasimtiobvrueetdsheevafeivcreeittshoerirmwe
dretparredssaivtieodni,sio.red.,efrowrithoasperwedhoomindaoncneootfsshtearreeottyhpeywanodrlrdigwidityhiontthheinrskinbguotr hoathveer their w
of fantasy, irrationality, or autistic thoughts. An increase significantly above 7 points i
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021,a1l8s, o572p5athological. Increased control of reality may, for example, be due to a11mof o20re severe
depressive disorder with a predominance of stereotypy and rigidity in thinking or othe
neurotic features, e.g., incredibly obsessive. Depressive and the lowest psychotic pro
nbeaunrdotsichfaedatuthreesh, eig.gh.,eisntcraevdeibralygoebvsaeslusievse.oDnetphriessssicvaelean. Ad tfhteerlofowreessttptshyecrhaoptiyc,ptrhoebraenwdsas a pos
hitaivdethienchriegahseestianvethraegeinvdailcuaetsoronofthtihs escdaleeg. rAeefteorffaodreaspt ttahteiroanpyo, fthtehrienwkiansgatpoossoitciviael reality
iwnchriecahseisininthdeiciantdeidcabtoyrcoofmthme doengarenesowfeardsa[p1t0a3ti]oannodf tchoirnrkeisnpgotnodsosctioaltrheealuitsyu, walhsiochciiasl rules o
isnodciiceattye,dubsyuacollmyma ognuanraswnteeres o[1f0c3o] nanfodrmcoirtryestpootnhdes etonvthireounsmuaelnstoncieaclersuslaesryoffosorchieutym, an inte
ugsruaatilolyna. guarantee of conformity to the environment necessary for human integration.
FFiigguurree44. .GGrarfa4f 4RORROfRorfocornctoancttawcitthwrietahlirtey.ality.
33..33..EEvvaaluluataitoinonofotfhtehHe aHndanTdesTt est
of resFFprrooonmmsetshthbeeeHfHoarnaednfodTreeTssettstrtheserueralstpusyl(t,psinr(ecpslureendstieendngtirenedaTciatniboTlneas2be)l,xepw2ree),snswoinetigcneinodtteiacrephdeigrashoehnriagflrhedeqisruteafrnneccqyeuency o
arnesdpaonnsinescrbeeafsoerdedfoegrereset tohferpaspycyh, oinpcaltuhdolionggyr. eaRcetsipoonnsseexsperxepsrseinssginigntderisptaenrscoeneaxlpdreisstsance and
naonn-inadcraepatisveedfodremgsreoef boefhapvsiyocuhroinpcartehaoselodgfyee. liRnegsspoofnsstreessse, xwperaeksnsienssg, odrisatvaonidceanecxeporfess non
iandtearppteivrseonfoarlmorseonfvbireohnamveionutarl icnocnrteaacstse.dTfheeesleinagnswofersstraersesc,hwareaacktnereises,dobryawvoeaidkaenecde of inter
cpoenrtsaoctnwalitohrreenalvitiyr.oAnmhiegnhtsaclocroenistaaclwtsa.yTshpeastehoalnosgwicaelrsanadrerecheacrtascptreorbisleemdsb[y6]w. Aeaftkerentheed contac
sweciothndreraoluintyd. oAf thesigtihngscinortheeisacatilnwgaoyust praattihoo(AloOgRic)ailnadnexd, aredfeleccretsasperionbbluelmlysin[g6,]t.hArefattesr, the sec
doensdtruroctuionndooffptreosptienrgty,inlyitnhge, aecstcianpgeso,urat criaatlidois(AcrOimRin) aintiodne,xs,paradyeicnrge,aasbeuisne bouf olltyhienrgs,, threats
pidinnretetosodtlliercerutraecandtncioaecg,enag, noraedfnsdspnieronotnhepo[te6hl,rio1stm0lyi4,s,wm1ly0ai5wsn].fgao,suenfsodcu.anTpdhei.ss,Tirhnadicseixainlidsdecioxsncirssiimdcoeirnnesaditditooenrbe,edsaptvroaablyiedinaign,vdaaicblaiudtosirenodofficoatthoerros
Table 2. Group averages forpTrheedHiacnteddTeasgt gcarteegssoiroiens r[e6la,1te0d4t,o10th5e].distance from reality and the level of pathology before
and after forest therapy.
Table 2. Group averages for The Hand Test categories related to the distance from reality and the level of pathology before
and after forest thCeroanptryo.l
Hand Test
Group
Hand Test
Non-adaptive
behaviour
Control
0.G38roup
Group A 1
Group B 1
Statistical
Statistical
Before
After
Therapy GrouTpheAra1py
Test Results
Before
Statistical TeTstherapy
After
GrTohueprapBy1
Test Results
Statistical Test
Before Ther- After Ther-2
0.51 ± 0.a0p6y 0.42 ± 0.a0p8y
p
t
<
=
0.0R1e*sults
6.35
0.62
Before Ther-
± 0.09apy 0.48
±
A0.f1t1earpTyhe2rpt-
<
=
0.01
5.38
*Results
Non-maPadanathiipfooetulsiotvragetiicboanelhs av-
0.910.38
1.39
±0.501.15±
0.06
1.18
±0.402.0±9
0.08
2
p
t
<
=
02.0p1<* 0.01
7.1t2= 6.35
*
2.01
±
00.1.672
±
0.029.01
±
00..1468
±
0.131zp==00.3.7324pt
<
=
0.01
5.38
*
Patholo(1tαgaVi=tacilao5u%lnesm)s.rBeapontrhiefseteesns-ttstwheeraeri0uth.s9em1dettoicdmeteeacnt 1s±t.a3ts9itsat±nicd0aal.l1ryd5sdigenviifiac1tia.on1nt8.d2±ifTf0e-.rt0eens9tcefosrbdefeop2reeptnad=<ned70n..at10fs2t1aemr*tphleesex(pαe2=ri.m50%1en)±.ta30lW.i1ni7tlecrovxeonnt2iom.n0a.1tTc±hhee0d*.1pc6haairrsactetesrt
3 p = 0.74
z = 0.33
1 Vmaelaunessarsetaptrisetisceanllyt tshigeniarciatnhtmdieffteircenmcee.an ± standard deviation. 2 T-test for dependent samples (α = 5%). 3 Wilcoxon matched
pairs test (α = 5%). Both tests were used to detect statistically significant differences before and after the experimental
intervention. The * character meaTnhseaesxtpaetirsitmiceanlltyalsiignnteifrivcaenttidoinffehraesnaces.tatistically significant effect on the achieved
final values, except for pathological manifestations in group B (p = 0.74). Statistically
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, x FOR PEER REVIEW
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, x FOR PEER REVIEW
12 o
12 of 2
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 572T5 he experimental intervention has a statistically significant effect on12thofe2a0 chieved
nal vTahlueeesx, pexecriemptenfotarlpianttherovloengtiicoanl mhaasnaifestsatatitsitoicnaslliyn sgirgonuifpicBan(tpe=ff0e.c7t4o).nStthaetisatcihcaiellvyedsigfi
nicaalnvtadluifefes,reenxcceepst(pfo<r 0p.a0t1h)owloegriecoalbmsearnviefdesitnatgioronus pinAgrinouvpalBue(ps =fo0r.7n4o)n. S-atadtaispttiicvaellybeshiganviif
siaicgnandnifitpcdaaitnfhfteodrleiofnfgecirecesanl(cpmes<a(0np.i0<f1e)0swt.0a1et)iroewnoesbr(esteo=rbv6see.3dr2vi;endt g=irn7o.ug1rp2o)uA, painnAdviainnluvgaerlsuofeuosprfnoBor innn-oavnda-aludpateipsvtfeiovberenhoanv-iaodu
bateinvhdeavpbioaeuthhraoavlnoidogupicraat(lhtom=loa5gn.i3icfa8el)s.mtaTathnioeifnreest(aitsti=onn6os.3s(2tta;=tti6=s.3t7i2c.;1at2l=l)y, 7as.n1i2gd)n,iainfnigcdarionnutgpdroeBucpirneBavsianeluvianelsuvfeaoslrfuonerosni-nadpaapt
isBFnnFtlnlFgneooiio—igiroovcpnggggooueia-iuiunnussracctbierirdhadpagtgeeehlola5,nnphi6lpm.6msioaiaftfFosgisgiiaviarvhicmihcrngincaeonoaoouaiineuofnwbulfwretpeeetmrpsssshs,ic6tr(ctgastaahttaohhonsnvthath=bimaiaiiiaofnfioooatne5ten,gucwnsn,g.aistepr3dinseasnn;8r((t;st(ottp)ittphot.hb=cthah=ahnhaTe=er5tesnea0eh,r.f0nrd3(e.ifie7pe.rni8gs7rr4si)ae=e4sagtt.)a;rhtr)Tn0preiietesnpei.hhia7nsfsaeeni4gisprrgrcir)gor,peresaenior,ttnecnsiiaohutfispictrhtugfeapceinlairferyaptciooiesiBrrncs,pueBaspt.titelptsibpTh.ayiaccsoTaetatBhaiahifirvatl.shibnelhcostTeyoiinoloiscpfhvostoaaasliaifegosialcntcatlgiiatgheyfcdccnnanaoiiteaossccidblaeliifttaogsgliixbeilgeccpnrstgxleaiahriepdctgarnerpaeabherdectmlnahpclmaeeedpproinnhecceehhedmtlroiaalycieceeydclmsrnealcaaeeeybnbrollsce,aelelmeareyiynabslell,osleoeauaiiiuwbelnnlswltil,suueianuitbnosgtnrisituotnvanrhitrgtttnorataevhhegaritulranteuedeortwphlodereuutsieiwudhsenesipBnpeescsiii—FsonenBeicngiFpodnauiangvirptdnteuheharorp5ii
agvvraeolruuagepes,;vseaovlmueenes;ipnervotehbniasinngdtrhsoisuagrper,otsuhipge,nrteihfeiacrreaenadtrleeyvdaieavbnioatvnietnidnandividvidieduxuatarllses.m. ely below otherwise averag
values; even in this group, there are deviant individuals.
FFFiigigguuurerree5.55T.. hTTehhHeeHaHnadannTddesTTtefesostrtfnfooornrn-naodonan-pa-tadidvaeappbtietvihveaevbibeoheuhar.vaivoiuoru.r.
FFFiigigguuurerree6.66T.. hTTehhHeeHaHnadannTddesTTtefesostrtfpfoaortrhppoalatohtghoicolaollogbgiecihacalavlbibeoheuharavalviopiuohureanrloapml hpeenhnae.onmomenean.a.
333.4..44. ..ETEEvovvalaaeullvuauataailottuinioaonotnef otothffhetethTheTheTehTmehhmaeetmimacataiAtctipciAcpAepArppcppeeppertrecicroeecnppetTptiieotosinnot nTTeTessett,stwe decided to use an interpretation
systemTToobeeefvovraaelluusacaottereintthghe.eITtThchaeenmmbaeatitcciocnAAcpluppdpeeerdcrectephpatittoiionnnpTrTeosebts,atn,wdwes ewdeditcheicdpiededresdotontaoulistuyesdeainsaonirndtieenrrtsperrpetraettiaot
aaaassaagnyynggngdssgdgdrtteprreepespermmssosrrisospvoiibbpeevpveneeeteefesfnnotointstersedryineinetetytdyndsosecctectdnyoonooercbrcdpiydiynyreneegbp1gbpsy.8rsy.r%eIi1eItos1t.8snsc8s%ic,a%oiaton.nhn.ne,b,btfehtreheecqecofuonrfenercneqlccuqluyuduedoenefdencrdycetyhsotphafootafrntiersnsieenpsspopdrpnooersncboersabesenaasddsneedsddceswrbcewryiatesh2iaet7shdp%eedpbrayesnbordy2sn7oha2%nel7itat%eaylrinotadyd-nisdhdoiershtdoeeerrtdore
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 5725
13 of 20
3.5. Evaluation of the Hypothesis
Based on the evaluation of projective tests by statistical methods, it is possible to
confirm that the experimental intervention in the form of forest therapy combined with
observational learning in groups “A” and “B” led to reduced aggressive behaviour.
3.6. Evaluation of Focus Groups
Results of focus groups discovered the attitudes to the given issues and the most criti-
cal aspects impacting newly identified knowledge. The alternation of agents’ statements
indicates group dynamics. Individual agents are marked with the abbreviation A + No.
(A32) “I understood that human babies and animal pups need the education to master
the rules in adulthood.”
(A5) “I felt sorry that squirrels also take care of foreign cubs when my mother did not
take care of me.”
(A18) “I understood it in mimicry; who wants to survive must adapt to the environ-
ment.”
(A17) “I liked how wolves could make friends with ravens; it is an entirely different
species.”
(A9) “I thought nature was cruel, and only the predator won. I did not expect animals
to help each other. I always thought that in nature, it works who with whom.”
(A21) “I was surprised that the bonds between the animals are similar to family life.”
(A9) “It seemed strange to me to compare us to animals, but finally, I enjoyed it.”
(A3) “Even in animals, there are inferior members of the tribe who get to eat only at
the end, which is similar to humans; when an individual is weak, he will never be the
pack leader.”
(A2) “I liked that even a tiny weak individual (tit) can protect species from a predator,
which can be much larger and more robust.”
Probands discovered some parallels between their own and forest animals’ behaviour.
For example, when a warden approaches, one warns the others. If they are caught in
the act, the youngest weakest individual is sacrificed because he/she receives the lowest
punishment. In case of damage, they try to camouflage it like that found in nature. They
can also come together as a pack and speak with one voice when needed. The crucial out-
come of focus groups is the finding that probands positively evaluated the compassionate
manifestations of animals and reported advantages, such as reduction of anxiety.
4. Discussion
Institutions providing substitutional social care are not a frequent place of research.
They are closed communities and are therefore demanding to obtain data. What is happen-
ing inside is not sufficiently researched. This research had the ambition to understand the
social reality better examined, even if only to a small extent, given its modest scope.
4.1. Evaluation of Projective Tests
TAT is more associated with social adjustment, and ROR more associated with thought
disorders. Both methods describe the personality in its entirety and can be used to assess
instinctual component, emotions, complexes or repressed tendencies. In the first round
of testing before forest therapy, a more significant increase in average values was found
for responses related to emotional instability and less controlled impulses in responses
involving animals, blood, and social reality checks. These interpretations are very often
associated with an excess of intrapsychic tension, acute distress and anxiety. Against the
background of tension, there are often insufficiently assimilated and integrated aggressive
impulses, easily activated by various irritating stimuli from the environment. Comparing
these research results with the conclusions of Gacono and Meloy [106], similar results were
obtained in the monitored traits (colour and chiaroscuro responses). This suggests the good
validity of this projective test, especially when assessing the affective component.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 5725
14 of 20
Similarly, in his research, Morávek [107] found statistically significant values (0.1%) in
the number of aggressive responses. These were primarily responses containing offensive
animals or humans. Interestingly, the Zw responses (interpretations of white spaces and
interfaces that can be considered a sign of an aggressive or negative attitude) proved to be
statistically insignificant, despite the literature and practice experience. In this research,
similar outcomes were found.
In the projective Hand Test, statistically significant differences between groups for
some items, including non-adaptive behaviour and pathological manifestations, were
observed. These categories were associated with an increased incidence of psychopatho-
logical features and weakened contact with reality. No significant values were found
in the predictor of aggression (AOR) or AGG (number of aggressive responses). The
aggressiveness index expresses the relative weight of socially positive cooperative atti-
tudes compared to directives and aggressiveness. Some authors, such as Klicperová [108],
Morávek [107], Volkova [109] and McGill [110], consider that this index is an accurate
evaluation of predicted aggression. In her research, Klicperová [108] focused on two moni-
tored traits, AGG-aggressiveness and CRIP-damage. She evaluated the answers containing
the monitored features on a three-point scale. So-called “percentage of aggression from
the obtained gross scores”, which had a significant value, was calculated. In his research,
Morávek [107] also focused on selected traits (AGG, number of aggressive responses; CRIP,
responses involving damage to the object; and DIR, responses involving directive direction,
regulation or control of others) and found statistically significant values on the scale of
aggression. Probands on the AOR or AGG scale did not achieve statistically significant dif-
ferences due to many factors, such as calming of the situation. Besides, the manifestations
of aggression change over time and circumstances.
In the Thematic Apperception Test (TAT), statistically significant differences between
the groups when assessing hetero-aggressive, auto-aggressive tendencies or topics were
not observed. Responses in which hetero-aggressive tendencies predominated were most
common in psychotic probands. Auto-aggressive tendencies predominated in probands
with personality disorders and depressed probands. However, activated defence mecha-
nisms that change the assessed patients’ experience and behaviour play a role, significantly
suppressing aggressive tendencies.
4.2. Evaluation of Forest Therapy
Forest therapy has been modified with elements from wilderness therapy and OBH,
Shinrin-yoku and forest pedagogy. We must distinguish between wilderness therapy and
wilderness experience programmes, boot camps similar to military recruit training in a
wilderness environment [111]. Actual wilderness therapy occurs under the supervision of
a licensed mental health professional (psychologist, psychiatrist) who works with partici-
pants and can provide individualised treatment plans regularly monitored and evaluated.
Due to the physical, cognitive, and social demands of wilderness therapy, this form of
treatment may not be effective with older adults, young children, or people with specific
physical disabilities. The approach may also be ineffective or unsafe for people experi-
encing severe or chronic mental health issues such as dementia, schizophrenia, and other
similar conditions.
Due to these limitations, OBH was combined with Forest therapy based on the Shinrin-
yoku methodology, observational learning and forest pedagogy. Our outcomes correspond
to scientific findings by other authors [55–61] concerning anxiety, depression, anger de-
crease, and mental well-being. In this research, the positive influence of the forest envi-
ronment on the development of potential acquisition of tacit knowledge was confirmed.
Contemporary studies of Shinrin-yoku present fewer participants’ results than this research
(12 male university students, 14 adolescent sexual offenders, 22 participants, 25 Japanese
students, 27 girls aged 12 to 14 years) [56,57,62]. Furuyashi [57] conducted a more extensive
study with 155 participants classified into two groups: those with and without depressive
tendencies. Shinrin-yoku therapy’s length also varied: three-day retreat [64], six times a day
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 5725
15 of 20
for 15 min [55]. Before and after forest therapies, reduced items related to psychopathology,
irritability, restlessness, emotional instability, egocentrism, relativity, and negativism were
evaluated.
4.3. Limitations and Possible Follow-Up Research
This research has potential limitations relating to the application of projective tests
application and the number of probands due to the institutes’ capacity. The question is
whether the Rorschach test reveals the patient’s psychological centre and the process of
their thoughts because probands can censor their thoughts before utterance. The evaluation
can also be skewed by the evaluator’s personality, classifying the patient’s answers into
predetermined categories. All three tests place significant demands on the experience and
correct interpretation of the person testing the probands, i.e., the evaluator (psychologist or
psychiatrist). The tests are sensitive to the immediate mental state of the subject, so a whole
battery of tests is carried out, which is evaluated by an experienced doctor-psychiatrist or
psychologist, who has the anamnestic data of patients in order to assess progress. However,
selected projective tests seem to be a suitable tool for predicting aggressive behaviour.
These methods can be recommended if trained personnel are used in a broader test battery,
mainly due to the test’s high reliability and validity (especially for detecting aggressive
behaviour). Therefore, based on a single projective test, there should be no clear conclusions
or even a diagnosis (for example, the test subject’s immediate mental state). Projective tests
should be compared with other methods (observation, questioning, objective personality
tests, etc.).
Focus groups also generate certain limits. Focus group participants can censor re-
sponses according to their demands; they can respond differently under the weight of these
circumstances than they would have done if they had been alone with the researcher. The
respondents may also conveniently conform to the majority’s opinion or, conversely, may
rebel against it. In both cases, however, misleading data could be provided.
Prior research studies relevant to this paper are limited (i.e., no study would show
the principles of cooperation to reduce aggressive behaviour on the examples of forest
animals). This limitation can be considered a challenging opportunity to identify gaps and
present further development in the field. This article aimed to compare the effectiveness
of methods and approaches to reduce the rate of aggressive manifestations, which is a
possible topic for further research via a longitudinal study with a more significant number
of probands.
This research is complemented by several studies that have already examined the
effect of wilderness therapy or Shinrin-yoku on reducing aggressiveness. So far, no study
has combined wilderness therapy, Shinrin-yoku, forest pedagogy, observational learning
and the use of projective tests in one piece of research, compared to previous ones. The
contribution of this research can be seen in the purposeful connection of the mixed methods
approach and testing probands before and after forest therapy by selected projective tests.
The paper’s novelty can be found because forest animals’ emotional life has become a
mirror with an educational aspect for young people with aggressive behaviour. Science
tends to degrade animal emotional manifestations as mere instincts [112], although several
scientific works are based on similar animal emotional expressions [113,114], covering only
selected partial aspects.
5. Conclusions
The importance of this research is evident due to the increasing number of aggressive
manifestations of adolescents. This paper aimed to verify the transformation in adolescents
‘attitudes and manners based on a mixed-methods approach and identify whether probands’
teamwork and social adaptation has improved while simultaneously reducing aggressive
manifestations. Thanks to the triangulation of methods, we obtained interesting empirical
material that would not have been reached by using only one data collection technique.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 5725
16 of 20
Projective tests provided information about the personality’s internal structure, balance
and maturity, perceptual-cognitive level, and reality contact.
The assessment or measurement of aggression is very complex. Projective tests evalu-
ate personality traits, reliably differentiating predispositions to aggressive manifestations
rather than aggressive behaviour as such. Projective tests are less transparent, so it is
difficult for the tested person to respond deceptively to cover up his/her antisocial and
aggressive tendencies. The results suggest that the combination of forest therapy and pro-
jective methods appears appropriate and complementary and improves clinical knowledge
in assessing aggressive behaviour.
These results can be relevant for policymakers and stakeholders involved in medicine
and education to utilise these proposals to design and develop successful strategies and
tools to promote this innovative approach. Forest animals showed adolescents ways of
communication, cooperation, and adaptability, care for others, i.e., characteristics without
which no community can work.
Author Contributions: Conceptualisation, K.M., J.Z. and R.D.; Methodology, K.M., J.Z. and R.D.;
Validation, J.Z., M.R. and K.M.; Formal analysis, J.Z., Z.V. and K.M.; Data curation, J.Z.; Writing—
original draft preparation, K.M. and R.D.; Writing—review and editing, K.M. and J.Z.; Supervision,
D.K., R.D., M.R., Translation, D.K. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the
manuscript.
Funding: This work was supported by National Agency for Agricultural Research of the Czech
Republic NAZV, grant numbers QK1920272.
Institutional Review Board Statement: The study was conducted according to the guidelines of
the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Institutional Review Board of The Institute of
Hospitality Management in Prague (protocol code HTV19-01 from 9 September 2019).
Informed Consent Statement: Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the
study.
Data Availability Statement: The data presented in this study are available on request from the
corresponding author. The data are not publicly available due to further evaluation within the
research.
Acknowledgments: We would like to thank M. Kadlecova, Psychiatrist, in memoriam, for her kindly
help by evaluating projective tests.
Conflicts of Interest: The authors declare no conflict of interest.
References
1. Bandura, A.; McDonald, F.J. Influence of social reinforcement and the behavior of models in shaping children’ s moral judgment.
J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 1963, 67, 274–281. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
2. Geller, B.; Greydanus, D.E. Aggression in adolescents. J. Adolesc. Health Care 1981, 1, 236–243. [CrossRef]
3. Cubbin, C.; Santelli, J.; Brindis, C.D.; Braveman, P. Neighborhood context and sexual behaviors among adolescents: Findings
from the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health. Perspect. Sex. Reprod. Health 2005, 37, 125–134. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
4. Routt, G.; Anderson, L. Adolescent Violence towards Parents. J. Aggress. Maltreat. Trauma 2011, 20, 1–19. [CrossRef]
5. Benson, M.J.; Buehler, C. Family Process and Peer Deviance Influences on Adolescent Aggression: Longitudinal Effects Across
Early and Middle Adolescence. Child Dev. 2012, 83, 1213–1228. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
6. Harsa, P.; Žukov, I.; Csémy, L. Possibility of Aggressiveness Evaluation by Means of Projection Tests. Cˇ eská A Slov. Psychiatr. 2009,
105, 20–26.
7. Rahayu, N.A.; Hamid, A.Y.S. Relationship of verbal aggresiveness with self-esteem and depression in verbally aggressive
adolescents at public middle school. Enfermería Clínica 2021, 31, S281–S285. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
8. Hutami, N. Death Instinct Manifested through Passive Aggresiveness and Its Social Effects in Melville’s “Bartleby the Scrivener”.
NOBEL J. Lit. Lang. Teach. 2017, 8, 1–8. [CrossRef]
9. Garandeau, C.F.; Cillessen, A.H.N. WITHDRAWN: From indirect aggression to invisible aggression: A conceptual view on
bullying and peer group manipulation. Aggress. Violent Behav. 2006, 11, 641–654. [CrossRef]
10. Wilson, L.; Mouilso, E.; Gentile, B.; Calhoun, K.; Zeichner, A. How is sexual aggression related to nonsexual aggression? A
meta-analytic review. Aggress. Violent Behav. 2015, 24, 199–213. [CrossRef]
11. Anderson, C.A.; Bushman, B.J. Human Aggression. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2002, 53, 27–51. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 5725
17 of 20
12. Miczek, K.A.; Almeida, R.M.M.; Kravitz, E.A.; Rissmann, E.F.; Boer, S.F.; Raine, A. Neurobiology of Escalated Aggression and
Violence. J. Neurosci. 2007, 27, 11803–11806. [CrossRef]
13. De Bono, A.; Muraven, M. Rejection perceptions: Feeling disrespected leads to greater aggression than feeling disliked. J. Exp.
Soc. Psychol. 2014, 55, 43–52. [CrossRef]
14. World Health Report WHO Home Page. Available online: https://www.who.int/whr/2010/en/ (accessed on 10 March 2020).
15. Morel, K.M.; Haden, S.C.; Meehan, K.B.; Papouchis, N. Adolescent Female Aggression: Functions and Etiology. J. Aggress.
Maltreat. Trauma 2017, 27, 367–385. [CrossRef]
16. Tripathi, N. Aggression among School Adolescent and its Association with Socio-Demographic Characteristics: A Cross Sectional
Study. Indian J. Youth Adolesc. Health 2020, 6, 7–13. [CrossRef]
17. Detullio, D.; Kennedy, T.D.; Millen, D.H. Adolescent aggression and suicidality: A meta-analysis. Aggress. Violent Behav. 2021,
101576. [CrossRef]
18. Kagan, J. Etiologies of adolescents at risk. J. Adolesc. Health 1991, 12, 591–596. [CrossRef]
19. Donovan, J.E.; Jessor, R.; Costa, F.M. Syndrome of problem behavior in adolescence: A replication. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 1988,
56, 762–765. [CrossRef]
20. Hamanová, J.; Csémy, L. Rizikové Chování v Dospívání a Jeho Vztah ke Zdraví; Triton: Prague, Czech Republic, 2014; ISBN
9788073877934.
21. Jessor, R. Risk behavior in adolescence: A psychosocial framework for understanding and action. J. Adolesc. Health 1991, 12,
597–605. [CrossRef]
22. Jessor, R.; Turbin, M.S. Parsing protection and risk for problem behavior versus prosocial behavior among US and Chinese
adolescents. J. Youth Adolesc. 2014, 43, 1037–1051. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
23. Costa, F.M.; Jessor, R.; Turbin, M.S.; Dong, Q.; Zhang, H.; Wang, C. The Role of Social Contexts in Adolescence: Context Protection
and Context Risk in the United States and China. Appl. Dev. Sci. 2005, 9, 67–85. [CrossRef]
24. Sasaki, S. Observational Learning and Conformity: Experimental Evidence. SSRN Electr. J. 2014. [CrossRef]
25. MacDonald, J.; Ahearn, W.H. Teaching observational learning to children with autism. J. Appl. Behav. Anal. 2015, 48, 800–816.
[CrossRef]
26. Tye, K.M.; Allsop, S.A. The Amygdala and Observational Learning. Sci. Trends 2018. [CrossRef]
27. Russell, K.C. What is Wilderness Therapy? J. Exp. Educ. 2001, 24, 70–79. [CrossRef]
28. Gass, M.; Russell, K.C. Adventure Therapy: Theory, Research, and Practice, 2nd ed.; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2020; ISBN 978-
1-130-58443-3.
29. Russell, K.C. An Assessment of Outcomes in Outdoor Behavioral Healthcare Treatment. Child Youth Care Forum 2003, 32, 355–381.
[CrossRef]
30. Wells, M.G.; Burlingame, G.M.; Lambert, M.J.; Hoag, M.J.; Hope, C.A. Conceptualization and measurement of patient change
during psychotherapy: Development of the Outcome Questionnaire and Youth Outcome Questionnaire. Psychother. Theory Res.
Pract. Train. 1996, 33, 275–283. [CrossRef]
31. Russell, K.C. Two Years Later: A Qualitative Assessment of Youth Well-Being and the Role of Aftercare in Outdoor Behavioral
Healthcare Treatment. Child Youth Care Forum 2005, 34, 209–239. [CrossRef]
32. Alvarsson, J.J.; Wiens, S.; Nilsson, M.E. Stress Recovery during Exposure to Nature Sound and Environmental Noise. Int. J.
Environ. Res. Public Health 2010, 7, 1036–1046. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
33. Pinto, R.; de Jonge, V.N.; Marques, J.C. Linking biodiversity indicators, ecosystem functioning, provision of services and human
well-being in estuarine systems: Application of a conceptual framework. Ecol. Indic. 2014, 36, 644–655. [CrossRef]
34. Jordan, M. Ecotherapy as Psychotherapy—Towards an Ecopsychotherapy. In Ecotherapy; Palgrave: London, UK, 2016; pp. 58–69.
[CrossRef]
35. Song, C. Physiological Effects of Nature Therapy: A Review of the Research in Japan. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2016, 13,
781. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
36. Browning, M.H.E.M.; Shipley, N.; McAnirlin, O.; Becker, D.; Yu, C.-P.; Hartig, T.; Dzhambov, A.M. An Actual Natural Setting
Improves Mood Better Than Its Virtual Counterpart: A Meta-Analysis of Experimental Data. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11. [CrossRef]
37. Lymeus, F.; Ahrling, M.; Apelman, J.; Florin, C.d.M.; Nilsson, C.; Vincenti, J.; Hartig, T. Mindfulness-Based Restoration Skills
Training (ReST) in a Natural Setting Compared to Conventional Mindfulness Training: Psychological Functioning After a
Five-Week Course. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11. [CrossRef]
38. Hansen, M.M.; Jones, R.; Tocchini, K. Shinrin-Yoku (Forest Bathing) and Nature Therapy: A State-of-the-Art Review. Int. J.
Environ. Res. Public Health 2017, 14, 851. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
39. Phillips, L. Using Nature as a Therapeutic Partner. Couns. Today 2018, 60, 26–33.
40. Ardoin, N.M.; Bowers, A.W.; Roth, N.W.; Holthuis, N. Environmental education and K-12 student outcomes: A review and
analysis of research. J. Environ. Educ. 2017, 49, 1–17. [CrossRef]
41. Kummer, H. Analogs of morality among non-human primates. In Morality as a Biological Phenomenon; Stent, G.S., Ed.; University
of California Press: Berkeley, CA, USA, 1978; pp. 31–47. ISBN 978-3820012118.
42. Broom, D.M. Sentience and Animal Welfare; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2014; ISBN 9781780644035.
43. Oschinsky, L. The Problem of Parallelism in Relation to the Subspecific Taxonomy of Homo Sapiens. Anthropologica 1963, 5, 131.
[CrossRef]
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 5725
18 of 20
44. Bekoff, M. Animal Emotions: Exploring Passionate Natures: Current interdisciplinary research provides compelling evidence
that many animals experience such emotions as joy, fear, love, despair, and grief—We are not alone. BioScience 2000, 50, 861–870.
[CrossRef]
45. Palagi, E. Sharing emotions builds bridges between individuals and between species. Anim. Sentience 2019, 3. [CrossRef]
46. Dawkins, M.S. Animal Minds and Animal Emotions. Am. Zool. 2000, 40, 883–888. [CrossRef]
47. Pavlík, J.F.A. Hayek and the Spontaneous Order Theory, 1st ed.; Profesional Publishing: Prague, Czech Republic, 2004; ISBN 80-
86419-57-6.
48. Kopcˇaj, A. ízení Proudu Zmeˇn; Silma: Ostrava, Czech Republic, 1999; ISBN 80-902358-1-6.
49. Kopcˇaj, A. Spirálový Management; Alfa: Praha, Czech Republic, 2007; ISBN 8086851710.
50. Rösler, S. Naturschutz am Ende. Naturwirtschaft als Zukunftsstrategie. In Land Nutzen—Natur Schützen: Von der Konfrontation zur
Kooperation; Evangelischer Presseverband: Karlsruhe, Germany, 1995; pp. 34–79. ISBN 978-3872101136.
51. Eisikovits, R.A. The Future of Residential Education and Care. Resid. Treat. Child. Youth 1991, 8, 5–19. [CrossRef]
52. Hewstone, M.; Stroebe, W. Introduction to Social Psychology: A European Perspective; Portál: Praha, Czech Republic, 2006; ISBN 80-
7367-092-5.
53. Li, Q. Effets des forêts et des bains de forêt (shinrin-yoku) sur la santé humaine: Une revue de la littérature. Rev. For. Française
2018, 135. [CrossRef]
54. Miyazaki, Y. Shinrin-Yoku: Lesní Terapie pro Zdraví a Relaxaci—Inspirujte se Japonskem, 1st ed.; Grada: Praha, Czech Republic, 2018;
ISBN 978-80-271-0778-0.
55. Morita, E.; Fukuda, S.; Nagano, J.; Hamajima, N.; Yamamoto, H.; Iwai, Y.; Shirakawa, T. Psychological effects of forest environ-
ments on healthy adults: Shinrin-yoku (forest-air bathing, walking) as a possible method of stress reduction. Public Health 2007,
121, 54–63. [CrossRef]
56. Hohashi, N.; Kobayashi, K. The effectiveness of a forest therapy (shinrin-yoku) program for girls aged 12 to 14 years: A crossover
study. Stress Sci. Res. 2013, 28, 82–89. [CrossRef]
57. Furuyashiki, A.; Tabuchi, K.; Norikoshi, K.; Kobayashi, T.; Oriyama, S. A comparative study of the physiological and psychological
effects of forest bathing (Shinrin-yoku) on working age people with and without depressive tendencies. Environ. Health Prev. Med.
2019, 24. [CrossRef]
58. Payne, M.; Delphinus, E. The most natural of natural therapies: A review of the health benefits derived from Shinrin-Yoku (Forest
Bathing). Adv. Integr. Med. 2019, 6, 109–110. [CrossRef]
59. Wen, Y.; Yan, Q.; Pan, Y.; Gu, X.; Liu, Y. Medical empirical research on forest bathing (Shinrin-yoku): A systematic review. Environ.
Health Prev. Med. 2019, 24. [CrossRef]
60. Kotera, Y.; Rhodes, C. Commentary: Suggesting Shinrin-yoku (forest bathing) for treating addiction. Addict. Behav. 2020, 111,
106556. [CrossRef]
61. Timko Olson, E.R.; Hansen, M.M.; Vermeesch, A. Mindfulness and Shinrin-Yoku: Potential for Physiological and Psychological
Interventions during Uncertain Times. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 9340. [CrossRef]
62. Kotera, Y.; Richardson, M.; Sheffield, D. Effects of Shinrin-Yoku (Forest Bathing) and Nature Therapy on Mental Health: A
Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. Int. J. Ment. Health Addict. 2020. [CrossRef]
63. Simonienko, K.; Jakubowska, M.; Konarzewska, B. Shinrin-yoku i terapia lasem—Przegla˛d literatury. Psychiatria 2020, 17, 145–154.
[CrossRef]
64. Kotera, Y.; Fido, D. Effects of Shinrin-Yoku Retreat on Mental Health: A Pilot Study in Fukushima, Japan. Int. J. Ment. Health
Addict. 2020. [CrossRef]
65. PAWS, Pedagogische Arbeit im Wald Home Page. Available online: https://web.archive.org/web/20150216214644/http:
//paws-europe.org/index.php (accessed on 13. March 2021).
66. Kuhleman, G.; Brühlmeier, A. Johann Heinrich Pestalozzi (1746–1827); Schneider-Verlag: Hohengehren, Germany, 2002;
ISBN 3-89676-536-1.
67. Cornell, J. Mit Kindern die Naturer Leben; Verlag an der Ruhr: Mülheim, Germany, 1991; ISBN 978-3834635679.
68. Cornell, J. Sharing Nature with Children; Dawn Publications: Nevada City, CA, USA, 1998; ISBN 0-916124-14-2.
69. Cornell, J. Learning Through Play and Experience; Portál: Praha, Czech Republic, 2012; ISBN 978-80-262-0145-8.
70. Bricklin, B.; Piotrowski, Z.A.; Wagner, E.E. The Hand Test: A New Projective Test with Special Reference to the Prediction of
Overt Aggressive Behavior. Am. Lect. Ser. Am. Lect. Psychol. 1962, 483. [CrossRef]
71. Clemence, A.J.; Hilsenroth, J.M.; Sivec, H.J. Use of the Hand test in the Classification of Psychiatric Inpatient Adolescents. J.
Personal. Assess 1998, 71, 228–241. [CrossRef]
72. Baity, M.R.; Hilsenroth, M.J. Rorschach Aggression Variables: A Study of Reliability and Validity. J. Personal. Assess. 1999, 72,
93–110. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
73. Svoboda, M.; Krejcˇírˇová, D.; Vágnerová, M. Psychodiagnostika Deˇtí a Dospívajících; Portál: Praha, Czech Republic, 2009; ISBN
978-80-7367-566-0.
74. Drummond, F. A Failure in the Discrimination of Aggressive Behavior of Undifferentiated Schizophrenics with the Hand Test. J.
Proj. Tech. Personal. Assess. 1966, 30, 275–279. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
75. Himelstein, P.; von Grunau, G. Differentiation of aggressive and nonaggressive schizophrenics with the Hand Test: Another
failure. Psychol. Rep. 1981, 49, 556. [CrossRef]
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 5725
19 of 20
76. Clemence, A.J.; Hilsenroth, M.J.; Sivic, H.J.; Rasch, M. Hand Test AGG and AOS variables: Relation with teacher rating of
aggressiveness. J. Personal. Assess. 1999, 73, 334–344. [CrossRef]
77. Lie, N.; Wagner, E.E. Prediction of criminal behavior in young Swedish women using a group administration of the hand test.
Percept. Mot. Skills 1996, 82, 975–978. [CrossRef]
78. Lecˇbych, M. Wagneru˚ v Hand test. Aplikace ve Výzkumu a Praxi, 1st ed.; Univerzita Palackého: Olomouc, Czech Republic, 2013;
ISBN 978-80-244-3862-7.
79. Kamphuis, J.H.; Kugeares, S.L.; Finn, S.E. Rorschach correlates of sexual abuse: Trauma content and aggression indices. J. Personal.
Assess. 2000, 75, 212–224. [CrossRef]
80. Baity, M.R.; Hilsenroth, M.J. Rorschach Aggressive Content (AgC) variable: A study of criterion validity. J. Personal. Assess. 2002,
78, 275–287. [CrossRef]
81. Krugman, J.I. A Clinical Validation of the Rorschach with Problem Children. Rorschach Res. Exch. 1942, 6, 61–70. [CrossRef]
82. Goldfarb, W. The Animal Symbol in the Rorschach Test, and an Animal Association Test. Rorschach Res. Exch. 1945, 9, 8–22.
[CrossRef]
83. Gacono, C.B.; Bannatyne-Gacono, L.; Meloy, J.R.; Baity, M.R. The Rorschach Extended Aggression Scores. Rorschachiana 2005, 27,
164–190. [CrossRef]
84. Geertsma, R.H. Factor analysis of Rorschach scoring categories for a population of normal subjects. J. Consult. Psychol. 1962, 26,
20–25. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
85. Exner, J.E. The Rorschach: A Comprehensive System. Basic Foundations and Principles of Interpretation, 4th ed.; Wiley & Sons: Hoboken,
NJ, USA, 2003; Volume 1, ISBN 978-0471386728.
86. Exner, J.E.; Erdberg, P. The Rorschach: A Comprehensive System, Volume 2: Advanced Interpretation, 3rd ed.; Routledge Chapman Hall:
London, UK, 2015; ISBN 9781138972872.
87. Cramer, P. Future directions for the Thematic Apperception Test. J. Personal. Assess. 1999, 72, 74–92. [CrossRef]
88. Aronow, E.; Weiss, K.A.; Rezinkoff, M. A Practical Guide to the Thematic Apperception Test; Taylor & Francis: London, UK, 2001;
ISBN 9780876309445.
89. Hibbard, S.; Mitchell, D.; Porcerelli, J. Internal consistency of the object relations and social cognition scales for the thematic
apperception test. J. Personal. Assess. 2001, 77, 408–419. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
90. Morgan, W. Origin and History of the Earliest Thematic Apperception test. J. Personal. Assess. 2002, 79, 422–445. [CrossRef]
91. Jenkins, S.R. A Handbook of Clinical Scoring Systems for the Thematic Apperception Test; Taylor and Francis Group: New York, NY,
USA, 2008; ISBN 1138873047.
92. Gruber, N.; Kreuzpointner, L. Measuring the reliability of picture story exercises like the TAT. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e79450.
[CrossRef]
93. Focus Groups. The SAGE Encyclopedia of Qualitative Research Methods; SAGE Publications: London, UK, 2008. [CrossRef]
94. International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems (ICD) Home Page. Available online: https:
//www.who.int/standards/classifications/classification-of-diseases. (accessed on 25 February 2020).
95. Pratt, J. Remarks on zeros and ties in the Wilcoxon signed rank procedures. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 1959, 54, 655–667. [CrossRef]
96. Berant, E.; Newborn, M.; Orgler, S. Convergence of Rorschach scales and self-report indexes of psychological distress: The
moderating role of self-disclosure. J. Personal. Assess. 2008, 90, 36–43. [CrossRef]
97. Kleiger, J.H. The Comprehensive System and Rorschach Performance Assessment System. In Rorschach Assessment of Psychotic
Phenomena; Routlege: London, UK, 2017; pp. 82–102. [CrossRef]
98. Bornstein, R.F. Rorschach score validation as a model for 21st century personality assessment. J. Personal. Assess. 2012, 94, 26–38.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]
99. Mihura, J.L.; Meyer, G.J.; Dumitrascu, N.; Bombel, G. The validity of individual Rorschach variables: Systematic reviews and
meta-analyses of the comprehensive system. Psychol. Bull. 2013, 139, 548–605. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
100. Giromini, L.; Viglione, D.J.; Brusadelli, E.; Lang, M.; Reese, J.B.; Zennaro, A. Cross-cultural validation of the Rorschach
developmental index. J. Personal. Assess. 2015, 97, 348–353. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
101. Tibon-Czopp, S. Rorschach Inkblot Method. In Encyclopedia of Adolescence; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2016; pp. 14–19.
[CrossRef]
102. Stuchl, V. Rorschach method as a direct Indicator of the particular drug Choice in patients with affective disorders. Psychiatr. Pro
Praxi 2017, 18, 84–86. [CrossRef]
103. Bohm, E. Lehrbuch der Rorschach-Psychodiagnostik: Für Psychologen, Ärzte und Pädagogen; Hans Huber: Bern, Switzerland, 1951;
ISBN 978-3456814506.
104. Panek, P.E.; Wagner, E.E.; Suen, H. Hand Test indices of violent and destructive behavior for institutionalised mental retardates. J.
Personal. Assess. 1979, 43, 376–378. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
105. Panek, P.E.; Wagner, E.E. Validation of two Hand test indices of Agressive Behavior in an Institutional Setting. J. Personal. Assess.
1989, 53, 169–172. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
106. Gacono, C.B.; Meloy, J.R. Rorschach Research and the Psychodiagnosis of Antisocial and Psychopathic Personalities. Rorschachiana
1997, 22, 130–148. [CrossRef]
107. Morávek, S. Aggression Diagnostic. Diploma Thesis, Charles University, Praha, Czech Republic, 2001.
108. Klicperová, M. Hostility Diagnostic. Diploma Thesis, Charles University, Praha, Czech Republic, 1980.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 5725
20 of 20
109. Volkova, I.V. Animal Cruelty as a Predictor of Aggression in Teenagers and Students; Department of Quality Assurance and Monitoring,
Russian Academy of Education: Moscow, Russia, 2019. [CrossRef]
110. McGill, K.A.; DiGiuseppe, R.; Zhuo, Y. The Code of Honor as a Predictor of Anger and Aggression. J. Aggress. Maltreat. Trauma
2020, 30, 226–242. [CrossRef]
111. Conner, M. Wilderness Therapy Programs and Wilderness Boot Camps: Is There a Difference? Home Page. Available online:
http://www.wildernesstherapy.org/Wilderness/WildernessVsBoot.htm (accessed on 12 January 2020).
112. Wohlleben, P. Das Seelenleben der Tiere. Liebe, Trauer, Mitgefühl—Erstaunliche Einblicke in Eine Verborgene Welt; Ludwig Verlag:
München, Germany, 2016; ISBN 9783453280823.
113. Massen, J.; Ritter, C.; Bugnyar, T. Tolerance and reward equity predict cooperation in ravens (Corvus corax). Sci. Rep. 2015, 5,
15021. [CrossRef]
114. Steiner, A.P.; Redish, A.D. Behavioral and neurophysiological correlates of regret in rat decision-making on a neuroeconomic task.
Nat. Neurosci. 2014, 17, 995–1002. [CrossRef] [PubMed]