The Psychological Effects of a Campus Forest Therapy Program
International Journal of
Environmental Research
and Public Health
Article
The Psychological Eects of a Campus Forest
Therapy Program
Jin Gun Kim 1, Tae Gyu Khil 1, Youngsuwn Lim 1, Kyungja Park 1, Minja Shin 1 and
Won Sop Shin 2,*
1 Department of Forest Therapy, Chungbuk National University, Cheongju 28644, Korea;
jingun0308@naver.com (J.G.K.); ktg0704@hanmail.net (T.G.K.); suwnmail@naver.com (Y.L.);
parkaous@hanmail.net (K.P.); yeamolove@hanmail.net (M.S.)
2 Department of Forest Sciences, Chungbuk National University, Cheongju 28644, Korea
* Correspondence: shinwon@chungbuk.ac.kr; Tel.: +82-43-261-2536
Received: 15 April 2020; Accepted: 11 May 2020; Published: 14 May 2020
Abstract: This study aimed to examine the psychological eects of a campus forest therapy
program. To evaluate these, pre-test and post-test control group design was employed. A total
of 38 participants participated in this study (19 in the campus forest therapy program group,
and 19 in control). The Profile of Mood State (POMS) questionnaire and Modified form of the Stress
Response Inventory (SRI-MF) were administered to each participant to assess psychological eects.
The results of this study revealed that participants in the campus forest therapy program group had
significantly positive increases in their mood and stress response compared with those of control group
participants. In conclusion, the campus forest therapy program is an ecient strategy to provide
psychological health benefits to university students and our study can inform decision-makers on the
priority of the campus forest program in societal eorts to promote psychological well-being among
university students.
Keywords: forest healing; campus forest; profile of mood state; stress response inventory; university
students’ stress
1. Introduction
Psychological health problems among university students is an important topic. University
students face many of the stressors, including academic demands, social challenges, and uncertainty
about the future, which are linked to increased levels of stress and psychological health problems [1,2].
Regehr et al. [3] reported that more than 50% of college students experience significant levels of anxiety
and depression. The American College Health Association [4], also reported in study results using
80,000 college students that 62% of students suered overwhelming levels of anxiety and 40% of the
students suered depression.
The college students with psychological health problems reported negative academic impact [5,6],
relationship dysfunction [7], high rate of drinking [8,9] and substance use [10–12], and high incidence
of suicide [13]. College is an important time, in which young people can adopt lasting healthy lifestyle
habits but is associated with increased chronic disease risk [14,15]. In the absence of a healthy means
to cope or proper support networks, this increase in psychological problems can be extremely taxing
on the body. It is therefore not surprising that maladaptive coping strategies and unhealthy lifestyle
choices are both prevalent and problematic in this population [16]. Therefore, it is important to cope
with psychological problems during the critical university stage. The use of forest and forest therapy is
increasingly recognized as an eective intervention for dealing with psychological problems [17,18].
Numerous studies have demonstrated the eects of using the forest in relieving stress levels and
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 3409; doi:10.3390/ijerph17103409
www.mdpi.com/journal/ijerph
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 3409
2 of 11
inducing psychological relaxation [19–22]. Spending time in a forest environment has also been shown
to enhance immune function by promoting the activity of Natural Killer (NK) cells [23], to reduce the
stress hormone cortisol concentration [24,25]. Visiting the forest is also increasingly recognized for
its potential to manage stress, and to promote mental, psychological, and physical health. Based on
the empirical research evidence, The Korea Forest Service (KFS) facilitated “Forest Therapy” to utilize
forests for enhancing people’s health and quality of life. The KFS has legitimized the concept of forest
therapy and launched a forest therapist system to develop and manage forest therapy programs [26].
“Forest therapy program” is a set of structured activities and cognitive-behavioral therapy-based
interventions using various elements of the forest environment to mitigate stress and to promote
health [27]. Regarding the psychological eectiveness of forest therapy programs, many previous
studies have reported improvement in depression, self-esteem, and anxiety [28–31]. For example,
Jang et al. [32] also reported that an after-school forest therapy program for infant participants was
eective in improving pro-social behavior and eciency of expressing themselves. For the oce
workers, Shin et al. [33] reported that the two-day forest therapy program provided significantly
positive changes in workers’ job stress and moods.
The campus forest represents a preexisting, accessible, and eective resource for enhancing
psychological health [34]. For example, Lee and Shin [35] reported that the forest therapy program,
performed at a university campus forest using university students, provided significantly positive
emotional improvement. Using 558 voluntary college students, Ibes et al. [34] also reported that
the campus forest provided a significant psychological impact on students, most commonly relief
from stress. Bang et al. [36] conducted a campus forest-walking program targeting university and
graduate students during their lunchtime and reported improvement in participants’ depression and
physical function.
To date, many empirical research results show that forest therapy programs provide a wide
range of psychological health benefits to the program participants [37–39]. However, few studies
on the psychological eects of campus forest therapy have yet been reported. Therefore, this study
was conducted to verify the psychological eects of campus forest therapy programs for university
students and to provide basic data for the development of various forest therapy programs for
university students in the future. Through this study, we hope to inform decision-makers on the
priority of the campus forest program in societal eorts to promote psychological well-being among
university students.
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
The participants in this study were 38 university students, 24 males (63%) and 14 females (37%),
with a mean age of 22 years. Recruitment posters were posted throughout the university buildings to
recruit volunteers. No incentives were provided to the volunteers. The inclusion criteria required the
participants to be current students at the specified university. More than that, participants who met the
following inclusion and exclusion criteria were considered eligible for the study: (1) no diagnosis of
reaction to severe stress and/or a depressive episode; and (2) could not be suering from any drug or
alcohol abuse.
We employed the “pretest-posttest control group design” and used a control group because it is
practically impossible to eliminate all of the bias and outside influence that could alter the results of
the experiment. To secure homogeneity between the forest therapy program and the control group,
the participants were randomly distributed into the two groups (i.e., 19 campus forest therapy program
group and 19 control group).
The experiment was conducted during the second semester of 2019 (September–November).
A total of eight sessions’ campus forest therapy program delivered by therapists was performed.
The participants were fully informed about the study’s purpose and signed an agreement.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 3409
3 of 11
This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Chungbuk National University
(IRB number: CBNU-201910-SB-945-01).
2.2. Experimental Sites
The field experiment was conducted in the Chungbuk National University campus forest in
Korea. The dominant species in the campus forest are Metasequoia (Metasequoia glyptostroboides),
Cypress (Chamaecyparis pisifera), and other broad-leaved tree species. The study area was a suitable
place for conducting a forest therapy program in terms of accessibility, distribution of a variety of
vegetation, and a low lope of topography. During the eight-sessions, the weather was clear, and the
mean temperature was 16.2 C ± 1.3 C.
2.3. Experimental Design
Forest Therapy Program
To collect data, an eight-session forest therapy program was performed. Once a week, each session
was delivered by a trained forest therapist for one and a half hours. During the eight sessions of the
program, participants were involved in many forest therapy activities such as forest dance, forest
meditation, forest exercise, walking, and others under the instruction of the therapist (see Table 1).
The forest therapy activities were developed and distributed according to each appropriate sessions’
theme based on consultation with researchers in forest therapy/forest recreation and forest therapists
(see Figure 1). The main theme of the program was to reduce stress and improve self-esteem for the
participants. The participants in the control group did not receive leaflets, lectures, or any forest
therapy activities and were asked to follow their routine activities during the experimental period.
Table 1. Themes and Activities of the Forest Therapy Program.
Theme
Rapport building
Stress reduction
Improvement of sense of
belonging and self-esteem
Cooperation and trust
Session
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
Program Activities
Ice breaking introduction; familiarity with forest; lecture on
stress management
Clapping exercise; Forest folk dance
Forest orienteering (using natural objects to solve group
mission); Physical stimulation for relaxation
Group gaming activities using natural objects (drawing natural
objects, hit the target with an acorn)
Forest exercise (Forest walking, stretching)
Barefoot walking in forest; Talking to Nature
Natural object five senses game; Photo healing (taking pictures
of nature and story-telling)
Forest band exercise; rope game
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 3409
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, x
4 of 11
4 of 11
(a)
(b)
FiFgiugurere11. . CCaammppuussfofroersetstthtehrearpaypypropgroragmraminteinrvteernvtieonnt.io(na). fo(rae)stfoforelkstdfaonlcke;d(abn)clee;ct(ubr)e loenctsutrreesson
stmreasnsamgeamnaegnetm. ent.
2.24..4.PPsyscyhcohlogloicgailcaMl eMaseuarseumreenmtent
ToTommeaesausurereththeeppssyycchhoollooggiiccaalleeffeeccttss ooff the experimmeennttss,,tthhrreeeessccaalelesswweerereaaddmmininisitsetreerdedtotoeaecahch
papratritciicpipanant tbbefeoforereaannddaaftfeterrtthheeiinntteerrvveennttiioonn..
TTooasassessessththeeeefefeccttoofffoforreesstttthheerraappyy oonn emotionaall ssttaattee,, tthheeKKoorreeaannvveersrisoionnoof fthtehePrPorfoileleofof
MMoooddSStatatetess(P(POOMMSS))wwaasseemmppllooyyeedd [[4400]. The POOMMSSiissaarreelliiaabblleeaannddvvaalildidininstsrturummenent tfofrorasassessessisnigng
pspyscyhchoolologgicicaallddisisttrreessss[[4411,,4422]] aanndd hhaas bbeen used prevviioouussllyyttooeessttimimaateteththeeininfluuenenceceofofbrbireifefofroersetst
exepxpereireiennccee[1[1992211]]aasswweellllaasslloonngg--tteerrmm ffoorreesstt tthheerraappyy pprrooggrraamm oonnmmooooddssttaatetess[4[433,4,444].].TThehePOPOMMS S
mmeaesausurersessisxixmmooododstsattaetse:s: tetnesnisoinonanaxnixeiteyty(T(TAA)”)”“dedperpersessisoino-nd-edjeejcetciotinon(D(D)”),,anagnegrerhohsotsiltiitlyity(A(A–H)”
“fHat)iguefa(tFig)”u,e’ “(cFo)”n,fuscioonnf(uCs)io”,na(nCd)”,vaignodr(vVi)go[r4(1V,4)2].[4A1,42v]e. -ApofiinvteL-pikoeinrtt sLciakleert(0sc=alsetr(o0n=glsytraognrgeelyto
4 a=grsetreotnog4ly= dstirsoanggrelye)dwisaasgruesee)dwfaosruesaecdh fiotermeatcoheitveamlutaoteeveaalcuhapteaeraticchippaanrttiscimpaonotd’ssmtaoteosd. sTtahteesK. oTrheean
veKrosiroenanovf ePrOsiMonSohfaPsOaMreSlahtaivsealyrehlaigtihverleylihabigilhitryel(iCabroilnitbya(cChrosnαb=ac0h.8s5α) [=405.]8.5) [45].
ToTommeaesausurereppaartritcicipipaannttss ssttrreessss rreessppoonnssee lleevveellss,, TThhee MMooddiifieeddfoforrmmoof fththeeSStrtersesssRResepsopnosnese
InIvnevnentotroyry(S(RSRI-IM-MFF) )wwasasuusesded. .TThheeSSRRI-IM-MFFwwasasoorirgigininaalllylyddeveveleoloppeeddbbyyKKoohhetetala.l[.4[64]6]anadndrerveivsiesdedby
CbhyoiCeht oail.e[t47a]l.. [T4h7e].STRhIe-MSRFIi-sMaFkiesyamkeeaysumreemaseunret mtoeonltwtoitohl rwesipthecrtetsopesctrtetsos,sptraerstsi,cuplaarrtliycutlhaerlmy ethnetal
hemaeltnhtaalnhdeaplthhyasnicdapl hsyymsicpatlosmyms pretolamtesdretloatsetdretsoss[t4r8es]sa[n4d8]haansdbheaesnbueesnedusperdevpiroeuvsiolyustloy etosteimstiamteattehe
etheecteofffefcotroefstfothreesrtapthyeprarpoygrparmogornamstroensss[t4re9s]s. T[4h9e].sTcahlee sacsasleessaesssepsasertsicpiparatnictipsasntrte’sssstrreesspsornesspe olenvseels
rihneahliltecsiielvagumaebhdli5slhri-inetpialnygsioac(iaslbnCuoi5tdlrm-ioLtipnynaiokgtb(iieCnazsrtcarothotLmsi’niocskabanαteali,rezct=aha(ns1t’0csigoa.=9αenl3er=s,,)0t(aar1[.no94n3g=n7d)e]gs.r[dtl4,yre7oapd]nn.ridgselasydsgeidropeinsreea.;sg5Tsri=ehoeens;.tS5rTRo=hnIes-gMtrSlyoRFnaIhg-gMlaryseFeaa)hg.traoTestehaa)e.ltToSohRftae2Il-2SMoRfitFI2e-2MmhaisFts,ehrmaeanlssad, traieevnlaaedctlhiyevaehictlieyhgmh
2.25..5.DDaatataAAnanlaylsyissis
DsocpUecorsSimeocAsTr-pedi)Thpn.aehettrmDieedsveooadespcgtacaiasrortrtaaci-atppoditcctlhieoilisvmeipltclceiaecotincenssgttdtfrescaoa’dfotrpoimmpsfrhtostiiapyrcchtrsciitiioshhnscnoseifosotdlaumorsnmgdtmpudiyacrdeiatowasiyluoenendtwesrcfe,oaefmesanmrtcendeateaasanolnvnydubsaazat,erlceryitsodadwztmbaeuedlndeeesedisnvnvu.aagisrTparidnShitraiePgeodb-SnpleSSeav,aPsn1iifS.arrd8TSete.id0hqo1peuWn8to-.p,est0ineatnf-sWricdtreteyseoqi,sdnwwutasdetnse-ontfrd(weoceSsyrPpsct,sSoee(aSnrwSanc,dPceeCdhSurnhSectpgi,tacceerCgoadorenghcuteodipotcn,ouatIccga(Lpotfgo,eomre,UderepIsStLtseaooAt.nre)t.
pathrteircaippaynptrsogprsaymchaonlodgcicoanltreolegcrtsoubpetsw). eAelnl sptraeti-satincdalpteosstts-twesetrsefuorseedacaht agrpo-uvaplu(feoorefs<t0t.h0e5rsaipgynipfircoagnrcaem
anledveclo.ntrol groups). All statistical tests were used at a p-value of < 0.05 significance level.
3.3.RReessuultlsts
3.1. Profile of Mood States (POMS)
3.1. Profile of Mood States (POMS)
The results of paired t-tests between pre- and post-tests POMS scores for each group are
presenTthede irnesFuilgtsuroef 2p. aAirsedcatn-tebsetssebeent,wteheenrepwrea-s aansdigpnoifistc-atensttsdePcOreMasSesicnorTeostafloMr eoaocdh Dgirsotuuprbaarnece
(spcps(orpcreroreeer2ssee22sn.f23ot.f73eord7r±c±aci3nam3.m.1F1p3pi3u,g,upsupsorfoeofssotrt2re11.es00sAt..t11tst1h1hce±±earrn2a2a.p.7pb7y3ye3,,gstgter=ro=eo4nuu4.,p3p.36tha68aef8,fttr,peeeprr=w=ee0iia.0gg0s.hh00a0t0t)0sss.)eeiT.gsshTsnsieiihoofreinncesrasseunoostlfuftsdflftooeosrcfreorepsfestaaptitsartheheierdeiernradtap-Tpttye-oytsteptapssrltorisMnogidgnroaridocamiadmctaeiDntdienitdsetthtreutvarhretvabntetahtntniehotcrieneeorne
5.90 ± 0.52, t = 3.067, p = 0.007),” and “vigor (pre 8.32 ± 0.71, post 11.00 ± 0.94, t = 2.661, p = 0.016)”.
However, no significant changes were found in the control group in Total Mood Disturbance
(pre 20.79 ± 3.12, post 21.84 ± 3.85, t = 0.355, p = 0.726), nor in all six sub-scales of the POMS: “tension-
anxiety (pre 5.95 ± 0.84, post 6.42 ± 0.87, t = 0.590, p = 0.563),” “anger-hostility (pre 5.79 ± 0.90, post
Int5. .J3. 7En±vi0ro.9n.2R, tes=. P0u.6bl7ic1H, peal=th02.502101,)1,7, 34d0e9pression-dejection (pre 4.26 ± 0.68, post 4.58 ± 0.74, t = 0.4805, opf=11
0.637),” “fatigue-inertia (pre 8.16 ± 0.53, post 8.32 ± 0.77, t = 0.221, p = 0.828),” “confusion-
wpp o=epbbsreeot0ewts.w4t0si.i0T9l3egd0.e7o9ne)n5±ritem,±tshc0te0da.en5.ene74tqtpt6w,lu(r,ypteiotvrsp==esagoil07r2oes.o..nn5i03tu2-c0i8dpv2e7±,ees,o’jpe0fcppc=.hp5t=rai04aeon.,r-06nttgp.0ie0c(eo8spi2sts)pr8”tiaes).n7cn”4.o,t1s.s3ri1ex7fas±o±sn.ru0gT0f.be6oh.7-r0res-9e,rhc,setaoptl=sowetthssietle0oirr3.tef2ay.24ptn6h4(yop,e±arpsPen0i=O.gd46n0.5Mc7i.,8of9tiS1nc=:±0atr)n2,o0t”t.el.2,5nd0ga5s7irn,fio,fodppeunrop“=-esVsant0,nic.gw20xe.o4si4eer17itp)(ny”p±e,r(rp“pfe0rof.rae14ret-m09it6g.e,3e.sut37dte7=±-sti±-cn40toee..0r62sr.et039tssi,82a,,
(pbreet7w.5e3en±t0h.e68g,ropuopsts,5e.1x1ce±pt0f.o6r4, vtig=o3r”.0s2u8b, -psc=ale0.(0t0=7)”2,.21c2o; npf=us0i.o03n3-b).eIwn ioldrdeerrmteonstu(ppproert7.s6ta3ti±sti0c.a5l2,
povsatl5id.9it0y±, w0e.5e2v, atlu=at3e.d06re7l,iapbi=lit0y.0o0f 7th)”is, saunbd-scavlieg.oCrro(pnrbeac8h.3a2lp±ha0o.7f10,.8p1o3sfto1r1th.0is0s±ub0-.s9c4a,letin=dic2a.t6e6d1,
p =re0la.0ti1v6e)ly. high reliability.
FigFiugruere2.2. TThhee rreessuullttssoof fpapiareirdedt-tet-stteasnt aalnyaselyssoefsPoroffPilerooflMe ooofdMSotaotdesS(PtaOteMs S()PsOcoMreSs). TscoAre=st.enTsioAn–=
tenasnixoinetya;nCxie=tyc;oCnfu=scioonn;fuAsioHn;=AanHge=r–ahnogsetirlithyo; sDtil=ityd;eDpr=esdsieopnr;eFss=iofna;tiFgu=ef;aVtig=uve;igVor=; TviMgoDr;=TTMoDtal=
ToMtaol oMdoDodistDuirsbtaunrcbea.nFcTeP. F=TFPo=resFtoTrehsetraTphyerParpoygrParmogGrarmouGp;roCuopn;t.C=oCnot.n=troCloGnrtoroulpG. *r*o*upp<. *0*.*00p1<, *0*.p00<1,
** 0p.0<1,0*.0p1,<*0p.0<5.0.05.
3.2.HMoowdeifvieedr,Fnoromsoigf nthieScatrnetsscRhaesnpgoenssewInevreenftooruyn(dSRinI-MthFe)control group in Total Mood Disturbance
(pre 20T.7o9 e±v3a.l1u2a,tepothste 2e1ff.8e4cti±ve3n.e8s5s, ot f=the0.p3a5r5t,icpip=an0ts.72s6tr)e, snsorresinpoanlsl esiaxftseurbt-hsecaeleigshot fstehsseioPnOs MofS:
“tecnasmiopnu-sanfoxireetsyt (tphreera5p.9y5e±xp0e.8ri4e,npcoesst,6a.4s2e±t o0f.8t7-t,ets=tsw0.e5r9e0,ppe=rf0o.r5m63e)d”,w“aitnhgperr-eh-oasntidlitpyo(sptr-tee5st.7S9R±I-M0.9F0,
posscto5r.e3s7. ± 0.92, t = 0.671, p = 0.511)”, “depression-dejection (pre 4.26 ± 0.68, post 4.58 ± 0.74,
t = 0.4F8o0r,thpe=pa0r.6ti3c7ip)”a,ntsfaintigcaume-pinuesrftoiare(sptrtehe8r.a1p6y±p0ro.5g3r,ampogstro8u.3p2, t±he0re.7w7,etre=sig0n.i2fi2c1a,npt d=ec0r.e8a2s8e)s”,
“coinntfhuesirotno-tbaelwstirledsesrrmesepnotn(spesre(p7r.e0057±.210±.524.,65p,opsots7t .4141.7±4 ±02.6.706, ,tt ==3.609.02,4p4,= p0.0=020).,8a1n0d)”a,llaontdherVsuigbo-r
(psreca1le0s.3o7f±th0e.6S0R, Ip-MosFt :9.9s5om±a0t.i7z6a,tito=n 0(p.5r2e22, 2p.7=40±.610.383)”, .post 18.11 ± 1.21, t = 2.903, p = 0.009)”, “anger
(prTeo14te.1s6t e±q0u.7iv0,apleonscte10o.f21pa±r0ti.c7i9p,atn=ts3.f9o8r0f,opr=es0t.0th01e)ra,payndanddecpornetsrsoilognr(opurpes2,0w.3e2 p±e1r.f2o1r,mpoesdt t1-6te.4s2ts
be±tw1.e2e0n, th=e2t.w86o6g, pro=u0p.s01p0r)”e-(tSeestesFciogruerse. T3)h.ere were no significant dierences in pre-test scores between
the groups, except for “vigor” sub-scale (t = 2.212; p = 0.033). In order to support statistical validity,
we evaluated reliability of this sub-scale. Cronbach alpha of 0.813 for this sub-scale indicated relatively
high reliability.
3.2. Modified Form of the Stress Response Inventory (SRI-MF)
To evaluate the eectiveness of the participants’ stress response after the eight sessions of campus
forest therapy experiences, a set of t-tests were performed with pre- and post-test SRI-MF scores.
For the participants in campus forest therapy program group, there were significant decreases
in their total stress responses (pre 57.21 ± 2.65, post 44.74 ± 2.76, t = 3.690, p = 0.002), and all other
sub-scales of the SRI-MF: “somatization (pre 22.74 ± 1.33, post 18.11 ± 1.21, t = 2.903, p = 0.009)”,
responses (pre 49.42 ± 2.90, post 52.05 ± 3.90, t = 0.795, p = 0.437), and all other sub-scales of the SRI-
MF: “somatization (pre 21.47 ± 1.37, post 23.74 ± 2.07, t= 1.320, p = 0.203)”, “anger (pre 10.89 ± 0.78,
post 11.58 ± 0.88, t = 0.646, p = 0.527)”, and “depression (pre 17.05 ± 1.19, post 16.74 ± 1.33, t = 0.317,
p = 0.755)”.
Int. J. ETnvoirotne.sRt eesq. PuuibvlaiclHeneaclteh o20f2p0,a1r7t,i3c4ip09ants for forest therapy and control groups, we performed 6t-otfe1s1ts
between the two groups’ pre-test scores. There were no significant differences in pre-test scores
“abnegtweree(pnrteh1e4g.1ro6u±ps0,.7e0x,cpepotsto1n0.2a1ng±er0.7s9u, bt-=sca3l.9e8(0t ,=p=3.101.090; 1p)=”,0a.0n0d4).dIneporredsesirotno (spurpep2o0r.t3s2ta±ti1s.t2ic1a,l
pvoasltid16it.y42, w±e1e.2v0a,lut a=te2d.8r6e6l,iapb=ili0ty.0o10f )thi(sSseuebF-isgcuarlee.3C).ronbach alpha of 0.730 for this sub-scale indicated
relatively high reliability.
FFigiguurere33. . TThhee rreessuullttssooffppaairiereddt-tt-etsetsat naanlaylsyesseosfoMf oMdoifdieifideFdorFmoromf tohfetshtreesstsrreesssproenspseoInnsveeInntvoerynt(oSrRyI-
(SMRFI-)MscFo)rsecso.rFeTs.PF=TFPo=reFsot rTehsterTahpeyraPpryogPrraomgraGmroGupro; uCpo;nCt.o=ntC. o=nCtroonl tGrorol Gupro. u**pp. *<*0p.0<1,0*.0p1,<*0p.0<5.0.05.
4. DHisocwusesvieorn, no significant changes were found in the control group participants’ total stress responses
(pre 4T9.h4i2s ±stu2d.9y0,epvaolsuta5t2e.d05th±e 3p.s9y0c,hto=logi0c.a7l9e5f,fepc=tiv0e.n43es7s),, easnpdecailallloythreergasrudbi-nsgcaelmesootifotnheanSdRIs-tMreFss:,
“soofmthateizcaatmionpu(sprfeor2e1s.4t 7th±er1a.3p7y, pporostgr2a3m.7.4 I±t 2re.0v7e,atl=ed1th.3a2t0,ppar=tic0ip.2a0n3t)s”,in“ancagmerp(upsrefo1r0e.s8t9 t±he0r.a7p8,y
pionstte1rv1e.5n8ti±on0.h88a,dt s=ign0i.f6ic4a6n, tply= p0o.5s2i7ti)ve, acnhdangdeesprienssthioenir(pmreoo1d7.0s5ta±te1s.1a9n,dpostsrte1s6s.7r4es±po1n.3s3e,st a=ft0e.r31t7h,e
pi=nte0r.7v5e5n)ti.on. The result of this study indicated that campus forest therapy provides improvements
in pTaortitceisptaenqtus’ivpaslyecnhcoeloogfipcaalrthiceiapltahn.tTshfoerrefosurelstst othfetrhaispystuanddy acolsnotrporlogvrioduepasr,awtieonpaelrefotormuseedcta-mtesptuss
bfeotwreesetsnttoheprtwomo ogtreouppsys’cphroel-otgesictaslcowreelsl.-bTehienrge awmeroenngousnigivneifirsciatnytsdtuideernetnsc.es in pre-test scores between
the groTuhpesr,eesxucletps toofnthiasnsgteurdysustba-nscdalien(tth=es3a.m11e9;lipne= o0f.0a0n4)e. xInteonrsdiveer tnousmubpeprorotfssttautidsiteicsapl vroavliiddiitny,g
weeveidveanlucaetefodrrealniadbisluitpypoofrtthoisfsAubtt-esncatiloen. CRreosntboarachtioanlphTaheoofr0y.7(3A0RfoTr).thAisRsTubp-rsocaploesiensdtihcaattedexrpeolastuivreelyto
hnigahturreel,iasbuiclhitya.s forests, reduces mental fatigue or psychological stress. A theory attempting to explain
4t.hDesiseceufsfseicotsnwas proposed by Kaplan [50]. According to Kaplan’s ART, prolonged use of directed
attention leads to fatigue of neural mechanisms. The recovery of effective functioning is enabled by
settiTnhgiss stthuadty hevaavleuacteerdtathine pkseyychpolroogpiecratliees,ecstuivcehneasss, esbpeeincigallaywreagyar,dinegxteemnot”t,ionfaanscdinstarteisosn, o,f tahned
ca“cmopmupsafroarbeisltittyhe.rTahpeyseprcoogmrapmon. eInt trsevreefaelredtotthhaotspeakretiycippraonptesritniecsaomf fpoursesftosrtehsattthtreirgagpeyr pinstyecrhvoelnotgioicnal
hsatdatseisgcnoifintcrainbtultyinpgostoitirveestcohraatnigveeseixnpethrieeinrcme o[5o0d,5s1t]a.tTesheasnedsstutrdeisessrienscpluodnseetshaofsteerofthKeaipnltaenrv[e5n0]tiaonnd.
TShoenrgeseutlatlo. f[5t2h]i.sTshtuedryesiunldtsicoaftethdisthsatut dcaymaplsuossfuoprepsotrthbeiroapphyilpiaro[5v3id] easndimhpurmovaenmeevnotlsutiniopnartyictihpeaonrtises
p[s5y4c]h; ohluomgiacanlshheaavltehs.pTehnet rmesaunlytstohfotuhsiasnsdtusdoyf aylesaorps raodvaipdteinagratotiothnealneattouurasleecnavmirpounsmfoernets,tysetot hparvoemoontely
pisnyhcahboiltoegdicuarlbwaenllo-bneeisngfoarmroelnagtivuenliyvefreswitygsetnuedreantitosn. s [55]. Evolutionary perspectives premise that,
The results of this study stand in the same line of an extensive number of studies providing
evidence for and support of Attention Restoration Theory (ART). ART proposes that exposure to nature,
such as forests, reduces mental fatigue or psychological stress. A theory attempting to explain these
eects was proposed by Kaplan [50]. According to Kaplan’s ART, prolonged use of directed attention
leads to fatigue of neural mechanisms. The recovery of eective functioning is enabled by settings
that have certain key properties, such as “being away”, “extent”, “fascination”, and “comparability”.
These components refer to those key properties of forests that trigger psychological states contributing
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 3409
7 of 11
to restorative experience [50,51]. These studies include those of Kaplan [50] and Song et al. [52].
The results of this study also support biophilia [53] and human evolutionary theories [54]; humans
have spent many thousands of years adapting to the natural environment, yet have only inhabited
urban ones for relatively few generations [55]. Evolutionary perspectives premise that, because humans
evolved over millions of years in natural environments, we are to some degree physiologically and
psychologically adapted to nature rather than to urban settings.
The rapid increase in the urban population worldwide is one of the important global health
issues of the 21st century. According to the projections of the United Nations Population Division,
by 2030 more people in the developing world will live in urban than rural areas; by 2050, two-thirds
of its population is likely to be urban [56]. Urban dwellers face stressful situations in their living
environments, such as work, school, and even home. Therefore, restoration from everyday stress is
essential for their healthy lives. Therefore, the psychological benefits of forest therapy through campus
forests are important, and the campus forest, which is easily accessible to students, is expected to have
very important roles in promoting psychological health.
The results of this study revealed that the eight sessions of campus forest therapy
program provided significant positive changes in the participants’ total mood, and other
mood states such as “anger-hostility”, “tension-anxiety”, “depression-dejection”, “fatigue-inertia”,
“confusion-bewilderment” and “vigor”. The results of this study are consistent with previous
findings using a diverse population of participants such as middle-aged males [37], adults [39], senior
citizens [57], and mental hospital patients with aective and psychotic disorders [58].
The advantage of explicitly studying forest induced mood is that mood has relevant and
long-lasting consequences on such things as the immune system, physiological responses to the stressor,
cognitive skills, and helping behavior. Thus, forest therapy and its consequent moods should be
considered as socially relevant and deserving of public attention, especially for university students
who suer and face many stressors, including academic demands, social challenges, and uncertainty
about the future. A forest therapy program using campus forests would be an eective and economic
strategy, in terms of time and money, to cope with such stressors for university students.
Positive changes in university students’ mood states provide benefits beyond “feeling good”.
According to Izard [59], mood state influences what is attended to in the environment and therefore can
have a profound impact on subsequent cognition and behavior. Mood change, and mood in general,
have physiological correlates. Mood is an integral part of many forest therapy studies [37,39,57,58]
and is likely to be a product of forest therapy experiences. The significance of mood was demonstrated
by noting the impacts of mood on cognition, behavior, and physiology These impacts include learning,
task performing, helping behavior, socialization, and health [60]. The benefits resulting from improved
mood induced by forest therapy experiences may be one of the major justifications to the university for
the expenditure of its resources on the provision and management of campus forests.
This study also found that the eight sessions of campus forest therapy program provided a
significant decrease in total stress responses, and other sub-scales of the SRI-MF such as “somatization”,
“anger”, and “depression”. This study also confirms the results of previous empirical studies
indicating the forest therapy programs’ eectiveness on stress reduction and coping with stress/stress
response [61–68]. For example, Song et al. [65] reported forest therapy was eective for female
nursing students’ stress reduction. Using participants who were oce workers [64], middle-aged
population [49], elementary students [66], and cancer patients [67], most of the studies have reported
participants’ stress level decreases after taking forest therapy programs. Findings from scores of
studies indicate that various stress mitigation benefits are consistently rated by forest therapy as
very important consequences of their participation. The stress response is the process whereby a
person responds physiologically, psychologically, and often with specific behaviors, to a situation that
threatens well-being and health [68].
However, the study had several limitations. Firstly, the participants for this study were limited
to healthy university students in their 20 s. To generalize the findings, further studies are needed
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 3409
8 of 11
using dierent groups of the population with dierent socio-demographic characteristics. Secondly,
this study was conducted in a campus forest to validate the psychological eect of forest therapy.
Eects according to the various characteristics of the greenspace must be examined in the future.
Thirdly, participants’ prior expectations, preferences for nature and experiences of forest may influence
the results. Fourth, in this study the control group conducted their usual activities. Some of the
participants in the control group may use forests for their leisure, and those experiences may influence
the results of this study. Therefore, further studies are needed with participants who spend time
in forests without giving them any instructions. In sub-scales of “vigor” (Figure 2), and “anger”
(Figure 3), there were significant dierences in pre-test scores between the forest therapy and control
groups. The dierences indicated that the participants in both groups had dierent baselines of “vigor”
and “anger” levels. These dierences may influence the results of this study. Lastly, we recruited
participants for this study as volunteers, using so called self-selection. So self-selection might influence
the results of this study for generalizability. These limitations should be considered in future research.
Despite these limitations, this study provides notable evidence of the eect of forest therapy in a
campus forest, which is easily accessible to students.
5. Conclusions
This study showed that campus forest therapy intervention provided significant psychological
eects. More specifically, there were significant positive changes in participants’ emotional states and
stress responses. These results of study indicated the campus forest therapy program as a strategy
to promote student’s mental health, thereby the eectiveness of forest therapy is suggested as a
complementary therapy in modern urbanized society.
Author Contributions: J.G.K. performed data acquisition, statistical analysis, interpretation of the results, and
manuscript preparation. T.G.K., Y.L., K.P., and M.S. were involved with the forest therapy program. W.S.S. had an
important role in the overall performance of this research, particularly in experimental design and research idea.
All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Funding: This research received no external funding.
Acknowledgments: We thank the forest healing lab members of Chungbuk National University for their help.
We also gratefully thank forest therapists for their valuable guidance.
Conflicts of Interest: The authors declare no conflict of interest.
References
1. Kadison, R.; Di Geronimo, T. College of the Overwhelmed: The Campus Mental Health Crisis and What to Do
about It; Jossey-Bass: San Francisco, CA, USA, 2004.
2. Eisenberg, D.; Gollust, S.E.; Golberstein, E.; Hefner, J.L. Prevalence and correlates of depression, anxiety,
and suicidality among university students. Am. J. Orthopsychiatr. 2007, 77, 534–542. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
3. Regehr, C.; Glancy, D.; Pitts, A. Interventions to reduce stress in university students: A review and
meta-analysis. J. Aect. Disord. 2013, 148, 1–11. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
4. American College Health Association. National College Health Assessment II: Undergraduate Executive Summary
Spring 2017; American College Health Association: Hanover, MD, USA, 2017.
5. Hartley, M. Increasing Resilience: Strategies for Reducing Dropout Rates for College Students with Psychiatric
Disabilities. Am. J. Psychol Rehab. 2010, 13, 295–315. [CrossRef]
6. Bruaerts, R.; Mortier, P.; Kiekens, G.; Auerbach, R.P.; Cuijpers, P.; Demyttenaere, K.; Green, J.G.; Nock, M.K.;
Kessler, R.C. Mental health problems in college freshmen: Prevalence and academic functioning. J. Aect.
2018, 225, 97–103. [CrossRef]
7. Kerr, D.; Capaldi, D. Young men’s intimate partner violence and relationship functioning: Long-term
outcomes associated with suicide attempt and aggression in adolescence. Psychol. Med. 2011, 41, 759–769.
[CrossRef]
8. Geisner, I.; Mallett, K.; Kilmer, J.R. An examination of depressive symptoms and drinking patterns in first
year college students. Issues Ment. Health Nurs. 2012, 33, 280–287. [CrossRef]
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 3409
9 of 11
9. Pedrelli, P.; Borsari, B.; Lipson, S.K.; Heinze, J.E.; Eisenberg, D. Gender dierences in the relationships among
major depressive disorder, heavy alcohol use, and mental health treatment engagement among college
students. J. Stud. Alcohol Drugs 2016, 77, 620–628. [CrossRef]
10. Halperin, A.C.; Smith, S.S.; Heiligenstein, E.; Brown, D.; Fleming, M.F. Cigarette smoking and associated
health risks among students at five universities. NicoTine Tob. Res. 2010, 12, 96–104. [CrossRef]
11. Primack, B.; Land, S.; Fan, J.; Kim, K.; Rosen, D. Associations of mental health problems with waterpipe
tobacco and cigarette smoking among college students. Subst. Use Misuse 2013, 48, 211–219. [CrossRef]
12. Keith, D.; Hart, C.; McNeil, M.; Silver, R.; Goodwin, R. Frequent marijuana use, binge drinking and mental
health problems among undergraduates. Am. J. Addict. 2015, 24, 499–506. [CrossRef]
13. Keyes, C.L.; Eisenberg, D.; Perry, G.S.; Dube, S.R.; Kroenke, K.; Dhingra, S.S. The relationship of level
of positive mental health with current mental disorders in predicting suicidal behavior and academic
impairment in college students. J. Am. Coll. Health. 2012, 60, 126–133. [CrossRef]
14. Nelson, M.C.; Story, M.; Larson, N.I.; Neumark-Sztainer, D.; Lytle, L.A. Emerging adulthood and college-aged
youth: An overlooked age for weight-related behavior change. Obesity 2008, 16, 2205–2211. [CrossRef]
[PubMed]
15. Reynolds, E.K.; Magidson, J.F.; Mayes, L.C.; Lejuez, C.W. Risk-Taking Behaviors Across the Transition from
Adolescence to Young Adulthood. In Young Adult Mental Health; Grant, J.E., Potenza, M.N., Eds.; Oxford
University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2010; pp. 40–63.
16. Schmidt, L.; Sieverding, M.; Scheiter, F.; Obergfell, J. Predicting and explaining students’ stress with the
Demand–Control Model: Does neuroticism also matter? J. Educ. Psychol. 2015, 35, 449–465. [CrossRef]
17. Tsunetsugu, Y.; Park, B.; Miyazaki, Y. Trends in research related to “Shinrin-yoku” (taking in the forest
atmosphere or forest bathing) in Japan. Environ. Health Prev. Med. 2010, 15, 27. [CrossRef]
18. Song, C.; Ikei, H.; Kobayashi, M.; Miura, T.; Li, Q.; Kagawa, T.; Kumeda, S.; Imai, M.; Miyazaki, Y. Eects of
viewing forest landscape on middle-aged hypertensive men. Urban. For. Urban. Gree. 2017, 21, 247–252.
[CrossRef]
19. Park, B.; Tsunetsugu, Y.; Kasetani, T.; Kagawa, T.; Miyazaki, Y. The physiological eects of shinrin-yoku
(taking in the forest atmosphere or forest bathing): Evidence from field experiments in 24 forests across
Japan. Environ. Health Prev. Med. 2010, 15, 18. [CrossRef]
20. Lee, J.; Park, B.; Tsunetsugu, Y.; Ohira, T.; Kagawa, T.; Miyazaki, Y. Eect of forest bathing on physiological
and psychological responses in young japanese male subjects. Public Health. 2011, 125, 93–100. [CrossRef]
21. Tsunetsugu, Y.; Lee, J.; Park, B.; Tyrväinen, L.; Kagawa, T.; Miyazaki, Y. Physiological and psychological
eects of viewing urban forest landscapes assessed by multiple measurements. Landsc. Urban. Plann. 2013,
113, 90–93. [CrossRef]
22. Lee, J.; Tsunetsugu, Y.; Takayama, N.; Park, B.; Li, Q.; Song, C.; Komatsu, M.; Ikei, H.; Tyrväinen, L.;
Kagawa, T.; et al. Influence of forest therapy on cardiovascular relaxation in young adults. Evid. Based
Complement. Altern. Med. 2014, 2014, 834360. [CrossRef]
23. Li, Q.; Morimoto, K.; Kobayashi, M.; Inagaki, H.; Katsumata, M.; Hirata, Y.; Hirata, K.; Shimitzu, T.; Li, Y.;
Wakayama, Y.; et al. A forest bathing trip increases human natural killer activity and expression of anti-cancer
proteins in female subjects. J. Biol. Regul. Homeost. Agents 2008, 22, 45–55.
24. Park, B.; Tsunetsugu, Y.; Kasetani, T.; Hirano, H.; Kagawa, T.; Sato, M.; Miyazaki, Y. Physiological eects
of shinrin-yoku (taking in the atmosphere of the forest)—Using salivary cortisol and cerebral activity as
indicators. J. Physiol. Anthropol. 2007, 26, 123–128. [CrossRef]
25. Sung, J.; Woo, J.; Kim, W.; Lim, S.; Chung, E. The eect of cognitive behavior therapy-based “forest therapy”
program on blood pressure, salivary cortisol level, and quality of life in elderly hypertensive patients.
Clin. Exp. Hypertens. 2012, 34, 1–7. [CrossRef]
26. Shin, W. Forest Policy and Forest Healing in the Republic of Korea. Available online: https://www.infom.org/
news/2015/10/10.html. (accessed on 2 February 2020).
27. Jung, W.; Woo, J.; Ryu, J. Eect of a forest therapy program and the forest environment on female workers’
stress. Urban. For. Urban. Gree. 2015, 14, 274–281. [CrossRef]
28. Shin, W.; Yeoun, P.; Lee, J.; Kim, S.; Joo, J. The relationships among forest experience, anxiety and depression.
J. Korean Ins. For. Recreat. 2007, 11, 27–32.
29. Cho, Y.; Kim, D.; Yeoun, P.; Kwon, H.; Cho, H.; Lee, J. The influence of a seasonal forest education program
on psychological wellbeing and stress of adolescents. J. Korean Ins. For. Recreat. 2014, 18, 59–69.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 3409
10 of 11
30. Lim, Y.; Kim, D.; Yeoun, P. Changes in depression degree and self-esteem of senior citizens in a nursing home
according to forest therapy program. J. KIFR 2014, 18, 1–11.
31. Park, S.; Yeoun, P.; Hong, C.; Yeo, E.; Han, S.; Lee, H.; Lee, H.; Kang, J.; Cho, H.; Kim, Y. A study on the eect
of the forest healing programs on teachers’ stress and PANAS. Korean J. Ecol. Environ. 2017, 31, 606–614.
[CrossRef]
32. Jang, C.; Koo, C. Eects of after-school forest healing program activities on infant’s pro-social behavior and
self-ecacy. Korean J. Ecol. Environ. 2017, 31, 595–605. [CrossRef]
33. Shin, C.; Yeoun, P.; Kim, Y.; Eum, J.; Yim, Y.; Yoon, S.; Park, S.; Kim, I.; Lee, S. The influence of a forest healing
program on public servants in charge of social welfare and mental health care workers’s job stress and the
profile of mood states (POMS). J. Korean Soc. For. Sci. 2015, 104, 294–299. [CrossRef]
34. Ibes, D.; Hirama, I.; Schuyler, C. Greenspace ecotherapy interventions: The stress-reduction potential of
green micro-breaks integrating nature connection and mind-body skills. Ecopsychology 2018, 10, 137–150.
[CrossRef]
35. Lee, J.; Shin, W. The Eects of Campus Forest Therapy Program on University Students Emotional Stability
and Positive Thinking. Korean J. Ecol. Environ. 2019, 33, 748–757. [CrossRef]
36. Bang, K.; Lee, I.; Kim, S.; Lim, C.; Joh, H.; Park, B.; Song, M. The eects of a campus forest-walking program on
undergraduate and graduate students’ physical and psychological health. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health
2017, 14, 728. [CrossRef]
37. Ochiai, H.; Ikei, H.; Song, C.; Kobayashi, M.; Takamatsu, A.; Miura, T.; Kagawa, T.; Li, Q.; Kumeda, S.; Imai, M.;
et al. Physiological and psychological eects of forest therapy on middle-aged males with high-normal
blood pressure. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2015, 12, 2532–2542. [CrossRef]
38. Shin, C.; Yeon, P.; Jo, M.; Kim, J. Eects of forest healing activity on women’s menopausal symptoms and
mental health recovery. J. People Plants Environ. 2015, 18, 319–325. [CrossRef]
39. Park, C.; Kim, D.; Park, K.; Shin, C.; Kim, Y. Eects of yoga and meditation-focused forest healing programs
on profile of mood states (POMS) and stress response of adults. Korean J. Ecol. Environ. 2018, 32, 658–666.
[CrossRef]
40. Kim, E.; Lee, S.; Jeong, D.; Shin, M.; Yoon, I. Standardization and Reliability and Validity of the Korean
Edition of Profile of Mood States (K-POMS). Sleep Med. Psychophysiol. 2003, 10, 39–51.
41. McNair, D.; Lorr, M. An analysis of mood in neurotics. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 1964, 69, 620–627. [CrossRef]
42. McNair, D.; Lorr, M.; Droppleman, L. Manual for the Profile of Mood States; Educational and Industrial Testing
Service: San Diego, CA, USA, 1992.
43. Kim, M.; Wi, A.; Yoon, B.; Shim, B.S.; Han, Y.; Oh, E.; An, K. The influence of forest experience program on
physiological and psychological states in psychiatric inpatients. J. Korean Soc. For. Sci. 2015, 104, 133–139.
[CrossRef]
44. Park, C.; Kang, J.; An, M.; Park, S. Eects of Forest Therapy Program on Stress levels and Mood State in Fire
Fighters. Fire Sci. Eng. 2019, 6, 132–141. [CrossRef]
45. Yeun, E.; Shin-Park, K.K. Verification of the profile of mood states-brief: Cross-cultural analysis.
J. Clin. Psychol. 2006, 62, 1173–1180. [CrossRef]
46. Koh, K.; Park, J.; Kim, C. Development of the Stress Response Inventory. J. Korean Neuropsychiatr. Assoc.
2000, 39, 707–719.
47. Choi, S.; Kang, T.; Woo, J. Development and Validation of a Modified form of the Stress Response Inventory
for Workers. J. Korean Neuropsychiatr. Assoc. 2006, 45, 541–553.
48. Im, S.; Choi, H.; Song, M.; Kim, W.; Woo, J. Comparison of Eect of Two-Hour Exposure to Forest and
Urban Environments on Cytokine, Anti-Oxidant, and Stress Levels in Young Adults. Int. J. Environ. Res.
Public Health 2016, 13, 625. [CrossRef]
49. Hong, J.; Park, S.; Lee, J. Changes in depression and stress of the middle-aged and elderly through
participation in a forest therapy program for dementia prevention. J. People Plants Environ. 2019, 22, 699–709.
[CrossRef]
50. Kaplan, S. The restorative benefits of nature: Toward an integrative framework. J. Environ. Psychol. 1995, 15,
169–182. [CrossRef]
51. Kaplan, R.; Kaplan, S. The Experience of Nature: A Psychological Perspective; Cambridge University Press:
New York, NY, USA, 1989.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 3409
11 of 11
52. Song, C.; Ikei, H.; Igarashi, M.; Takagaki, M.; Miyazaki, Y. Physiological and psychological eects of a walk
in urban parks in fall. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2015, 12, 14216–14228. [CrossRef]
53. Wilson, E.O. Biophilia; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1984.
54. Ulrich, R.S. Natural versus urban scenes: Some psychophysiological eects. Environ. Behav. 1981, 13, 523–556.
[CrossRef]
55. Maller, C.; Townsend, M.; Pryor, A.; Brown, P.; St Leger, L. Healthy Nature Healthy People: ‘Contact with
Nature’ as an Upstream Health Promotion Intervention for Populations. Health Promot. Int. 2006, 21, 45–54.
[CrossRef]
56. Srivastava, K. Urbanization and mental health. Ind. Psychiatry. J. 2009, 18, 75. [CrossRef]
57. Yu, C.; Lin, C.; Tsai, M.; Tsai, Y.; Chen, C. Eects of short forest bathing program on autonomic nervous
system activity and mood states in middle-aged and elderly individuals. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health
2017, 14, 897. [CrossRef]
58. Bielinis, E.; Jaroszewska, A.; Łukowski, A.; Takayama, N. The Eects of a Forest Therapy Programme on
Mental Hospital Patients with Aective and Psychotic Disorders. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020,
17, 118. [CrossRef]
59. Izard, C.; Kagan, J.; Zajonc, R. (Eds.) Emotions, Cognition and Behavior; Cambridge University Press: New York,
NY, USA, 1984; pp. 17–37.
60. Hull, R.B., IV. Mood as a product of leisure: Causes and consequences. J. Leis. Res. 1990, 22, 99–111.
[CrossRef]
61. Song, C.; Ikei, H.; Igarashi, M.; Miwa, M.; Takagaki, M.; Miyazaki, Y. Physiological and psychological
responses of young males during spring-time walks in urban parks. J. Physiol. Anthropol. 2014, 33, 8.
[CrossRef]
62. Lee, B.; Lee, H. Eects of occupational and social stresses after forest therapy. J. Nat. Med. 2013, 2, 108–114.
63. Jang, S.; Koo, C.; Joo, S. Eects of forest prenatal education program on stress and emotional stability of
pregnant women. J. Korean Soc. People Plant. Environ. 2014, 17, 335–341. [CrossRef]
64. Lee, J.; Yeon, P.; Park, S.; Kang, J. Eects of Forest Therapy Programs on the Stress and Emotional Change of
Emotional Labor Workers. Korean Inst. For. Recreat. Welf. 2018, 22, 15–22.
65. Song, J.; Cha, J.; Lee, C.; Choi, Y.; Yeoun, P. Eects of forest healing program on stress response and spirituality
in female nursing college students and there experience. J. KIFR 2014, 18, 109–125.
66. Morita, E.; Nagano, J.; Fukuda, S.; Nakashima, T.; Iwai, Y.; Yamamoto, H.; Hamajima, N. Relationship
between forest walking (shinrin-yoku) frequency and self-rated health status: Cross-sectional study of
healthy Japanese. Jpn. J. Biometeorol. 2009, 46, 99–107.
67. Nakau, M.; Imanishi, J.; Imanishi, J.; Watanabe, S.; Imanishi, A.; Baba, T.; Hirai, K.; Ito, T.; Chiba, W.;
Morimoto, Y. Spiritual care of cancer patients by integrated medicine in urban green space: A pilot study.
Explore 2013, 9, 87–90. [CrossRef]
68. Baum, A.; Fleming, R.; Singer, J.E. Understanding environmental stress: Strategies for conceptual and
methodological integration. Adv. Environ. Psychol. 1985, 5, 185–205.
© 2020 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access
article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution
(CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).