A Thematic Analysis of Multiple Pathways Between Nature Engagement Activities and Well-Being
ORIGINAL RESEARCH
published: 26 March 2021
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.580992
A Thematic Analysis of Multiple
Pathways Between Nature
Engagement Activities and
Well-Being
Anam Iqbal and Warren Mansell*
Faculty of Biology, Medicine and Health, School of Health Sciences, University of Manchester, Manchester, United Kingdom
Edited by:
Eric Brymer,
Australian College of Applied
Psychology (ACAP), Australia
Reviewed by:
Kathryn Jane Hoffmann Williams,
The University of Melbourne, Australia
Sonia Maria Guedes Gondim,
Federal University of Bahia, Brazil
*Correspondence:
Warren Mansell
warren.mansell@manchester.ac.uk
Specialty section:
This article was submitted to
Environmental Psychology,
a section of the journal
Frontiers in Psychology
Received: 07 July 2020
Accepted: 26 February 2021
Published: 26 March 2021
Citation:
Iqbal A and Mansell W (2021) A
Thematic Analysis of Multiple
Pathways Between Nature
Engagement Activities and Well-Being.
Front. Psychol. 12:580992.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.580992
Research studies have identified various different mechanisms in the effects of nature
engagement on well-being and mental health. However, rarely are multiple pathways
examined in the same study and little use has been made of first-hand, experiential
accounts through interviews. Therefore, a semi-structured interview was conducted with
seven female students who identified the role of nature engagement in their well-being
and mental health. After applying thematic analysis, 11 themes were extracted from
the data set, which were: “enjoying the different sensory input,” “calm nature facilitates
a calm mood,” “enhancing decision making and forming action plans,” “enhancing
efficiency and productivity,” “alleviating pressure from society’s expectations regarding
education,” “formation of community relations,” “nature puts things into perspective,”
“liking the contrast from the urban environment,” “feel freedom,” “coping mechanism,”
and “anxious if prevented or restricted.” The results indicate complementary mechanisms
for how nature-related activities benefit mental health and well-being that may occupy
different levels of experience within a hierarchical framework informed by perceptual
control theory.
Keywords: nature engagement, eco psychology, mental health, environment, well-being, mechanisms of change
INTRODUCTION
It has become increasingly recognized that nature engagement activities provide a convenient,
effective, community-wide means to support mental health and well-being, especially during
childhood (Bragg and Atkins, 2016). A recent Delphi study of 19 experts from seven countries
collated the diverse range of nature engagement interventions and their potential well-being and
health outcomes (Shanahan et al., 2019). They encompass ways to change people’s surroundings
(e.g., parks, workplaces), and widespread types of activities (e.g., adventure sports, wilderness play,
outdoor therapy). Well-being has been conceptualized from a range of perspectives, including
subjective well-being (Diener et al., 1999), social well-being (Larson, 1993), and psychological
well-being (Ryff and Keyes, 1995). Each of these perspectives on well-being presents multifaceted
constructs. For example, subjective well-being includes the phenomena of pleasant and unpleasant
affect, life satisfaction and satisfaction in specific domains (e.g., work, family, health) (Diener
et al., 1999). Social well-being combines social adjustment (including relationship satisfaction) and
social support from one’s network of contacts (Larson, 1993). Psychological well-being is also a
multifaceted construct (Ryff and Keyes, 1995) and includes autonomy, environmental mastery,
personal growth, positive relations, purpose in life and self-acceptance; these elements are generally
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org
1
March 2021 | Volume 12 | Article 580992
Iqbal and Mansell
Nature Engagement Pathways
considered to hold value (also known as eudaimonic well-being)
but not necessarily pleasure (hedonic well-being). A wide range
of inter-related mechanisms through which nature engagement
may have its effects on well-being have been put forward, but
rarely are they studied together, nor have the mechanisms been
analyzed in the context of different perspectives on well-being.
The current study aimed to discover the first-hand experience
of potential mechanisms by interviewing seven people who
identified as utilizing nature engagement for their well-being
and mental health. We did not impose a specific well-being
perspective but allowed participants to generate the facets of
well-being that were meaningful to them.
A number of theories are grounded in evolution theory,
pointing to the natural affinity of humans for natural
environments, animals and plants—commonly described as
the biophilia hypothesis (Wilson, 1984; Kellert and Wilson,
1995; Ewert et al., 2014). The sensory input from nature
itself may reduce anxiety, owing to an innate drive to seek
sensory experiences that eminate from nature (Ewert et al.,
2014). These experiential properties may allow the individual
to meet fundamental needs (Landon et al., 2020) such as
autonomy, competence and relatedness, as specified by Self-
Determination Theory (SDT; Deci and Ryan, 1985). The visual
qualities of natural scenes are the focus of Stress Reduction
Theory (Ulrich, 1983; Ulrich et al., 1991) that specifies the
“preferenda”—the aesthetic qualities of natural scenes—that
entail specific affective, cognitive and physiological states.
These include the textures, depth cues, level of complexity, and
configurations within a natural environment, and they engage
specific psychoevolutionary modes, such as the pleasant affect
linked to exploration and approach throughout an environment
containing depth and complexity.
There is consistent evidence from early studies that the visual
perception of natural environments can improve indices of well-
being, such as reduced anxiety, pro-social behavior and reduced
physiological symptoms of stress (Berto, 2014). A meta-analysis
of 32 studies found that contact with natural environments was
associated with a moderate increase positive affect and a smaller,
but statistically robust, decrease in negative affect (McMahan
and Estes, 2015). Specifically, there is emerging evidence for the
proposal that it is the fractal geometry of visual scenes of nature
that is unique, provides aesthetic pleasure, and may specifically
engage this mode of attention (Purcell et al., 2001). In turn,
it is proposed that when a person becomes aware of these
perceptual qualities of nature, they experience a connectedness
to nature that is part of their identity (Heerwagen and Orians,
2002; Schultz, 2002). The Connectedness to Nature Scale (CNS)
is a standardized self-report scale to assess this construct (Mayer
and Frantz, 2004). It correlates with overall life satisfaction and
various facets of well-being (Mayer and Frantz, 2004; Howell
et al., 2011; Wolsko and Lindberg, 2013). Consistent with this
view, engagement with natural beauty has been found to mediate
the relationship between nature connectedness and well-being
(Zhang et al., 2014).
Attention restoration theory (ART) suggests that spending
time perceiving nature helps to improve focus and concentration
levels through replenish an attentional resource required to
focus and inhibit distractions (Kaplan and Kaplan, 1989; Kaplan,
1995). Emerging evidence from the beneficial effects of nature on
cognitive task performance supports this proposal (Berto, 2014),
including a meta-analysis of 42 studies in which improvements
in working memory and cognitive flexibility were identified
(Stevenson et al., 2018). It is claimed that nature engagement
creates these effects by engaging a model of attention known
as “fascination” that is effortless, thereby allowing attentional
resource to be restored.
A distinct but related approach points to the role of nature
engagement in helping to balance the biological systems for
managing threat, pursuing reward and seeking comfort to restore
homeostasis and allow self-reflection (Richardson, 2019). This
approach links nature engagement to the understanding of
compassion and mindfulness. For example, there is also emerging
evidence that nature connectedness correlates with other factors
involving attention–particularly mindfulness, as supported by a
recent meta-analysis of 12 studies (Schutte and Malouff, 2018).
The relationships vary depending on the facet of mindfulness
assessed, and there are indications that reflective self-attention
may indeed be the closer correlate (Richardson and Sheffield,
2015). The self-regulation approach builds on evidence of this
kind, in describing the processes through which many aspects
of the self—mastery, attitudes, values—are regulated (Korpela,
1995). Yet a further strand of research goes beyond self-
reflection, to assess the role of awareness of a superordinate
perspective on the world, akin to mysticism or spirituality
(ecological-self theory, Bragg, 1996; transpersonal psychology,
Ferrer, 2002; general sense of connectedness, Passmore and
Holder, 2017), and there is evidence that this also mediates
between connection to nature and well-being (Kamitsis and
Francis, 2013; Trigwell et al., 2014). Most recently, drawing upon
Gibson’s work (e.g., Gibson, 1979), an ecological stance toward
nature connection has been described to explain the capacity
for nature to enable healthy actions through its wide variety
of affordances and niches, such as trees to climb and a wide
expanse to explore (Araújo et al., 2019; Brymer et al., 2020).
This ecological stance has been encapsulated within a broader
humanistic and existential approach to explain, for example, the
benefits of extreme sports in natural environments (Immonen
et al., 2018). A related conceptualization utilizes attachment
theory and phenomenology to describe the way that people’s
fundamental relationships with nature (e.g., as a secure base; as
an embodied interaction) contribute to, and support, the sense of
self (Schweitzer et al., 2018).
In order to attempt to organize this literature, Figure 1
displays the main theories of nature engagement alongside the
main mechanisms of action that they specify. Provisionally, these
can be organized in two dimensions to show their approximate
relationship. They vary in the level of process that they tend to
describe–sensimotor control, attention, self, spiritual—and they
vary in terms of the degree to which their mechanisms are specific
to nature or more widely applicable. There is however no formal
assessment of their overlap, and it is the aim of the current study
to focus on specifying mechanisms from the perspective of the
individual engaging with nature.
The research literature clearly points to a number of
mechanisms. Whilst theories such as ART and ecological-self
theory clearly presume a complex inter-relationship between
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org
2
March 2021 | Volume 12 | Article 580992
Iqbal and Mansell
Nature Engagement Pathways
FIGURE 1 | A visualization of the relationship between theories of the well-being benefits of nature engagement. The x-axis represents the degree to which the theory
tends to focus on mechanisms that are specific to the biological affinity of humans with natural environments (e.g., evolved biological systems, ecological identity), vs.
more general psychological mechanisms that are nonetheless facilitated by nature (e.g., attention, self-regulation), and the y-axis represents the level of abstraction of
the components of the theory, from sensimotor processes, through attentional, personal (self-related), and spiritual (transpersonal) components. The
ecological-existential/phenomenological-psychoanalytical approach by Brymer et al. encompasses the full spectrum of both axes.
mechanisms at multiple levels—sensory, attentional, self-
concept, spiritual—this provides a challenge for quantitative
research and statistical modeling. In contrast, this challenge may
be addressed by an interdisciplinary framework grounded in
biology and engineering, known as perceptual control theory
(PCT; Powers et al., 1960a,b; Powers, 1973, 2008). It may
be more productive to utilize a theory that is universal and
interdisciplinary so that: (a) the biopsychosocial domains of
nature engagement can be integrated, and (b) the benefits of
nature engagement can be integrated with other interventions
such as psychological therapy (e.g., Greenleaf et al., 2014; Jordan,
2014), sports (Barton and Pretty, 2010; Allan et al., 2020), and
the arts (Case, 2005).
PCT proposes that organisms control their sensory input
through a process that is an extension of homeostasis, mediated
externally. Humans, and other organisms, achieve and maintain
internal reference values for sensory input at their desired
states by acting against disturbances in the environment. These
controlled perceptions of the self and the environment are
organized hierarchically such that more abstract, long-term
experiences are maintained through setting the reference values
of the next level down. For example, a person who wants to
live a worthwhile life may uphold principles regarding their
affinity with nature, which entails that they go for regular hikes,
during which time they search out and attend to configurations
of the natural world that they find calming and pleasurable.
Table 1 illustrates 11 levels of perception in PCT and provides
examples within nature engagement. Note that in PCT, action
is used to control sensory input via “feedback functions” in the
environment; this contrasts with the view that affordances in the
environment provide sensory input to drive action, as proposed
within ecological psychology (Gibson, 1979).
PCT proposes that chronic disruptions in well-being can
emerge from internal conflict; this is the attempt to make
an experience match two or more, incompatible reference
values. When this occurs, attention needs to be directed and
sustained above the systems in conflict so that an intrinsic,
trial-and-error learning process, known as reorganization, can
adjust neural strengths and connections until the conflict
is resolved. Thus, PCT provides a biologically grounded
framework for human decision making, conflict resolution,
well-being enhancement and maintenance (Carey et al., 2014);
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org
3
March 2021 | Volume 12 | Article 580992
Iqbal and Mansell
Nature Engagement Pathways
TABLE 1 | The perceptual levels within PCT (Powers, 1998) and examples of how
they may be involved during nature engagement.
Name of Level Definition
The perceptual levels
within one example of
nature engagement
System concepts
Principles
Programs
Sequences
Categories
Relationships
Events
Transitions
Configurations
Sensations
Intensities
Conceptual organizations of
principles
Abstracted rules, values and
standards
Nested structures of test
and choice points regarding
sequences
Experiences that occur in a
specific temporal order
Classes of perceptions that
have shared characteristics
The ways in which two or
more lower level perceptions
relate to one another
A short, immediate,
succession of transitions
A change in configuration
over time, e.g., closing,
extending
A combination of sensations
that is perceived as a
distinct object
A vector of intensities
A signal directly from a
sensory organ
Being a nature lover
Getting the most of being
outdoors
Tree climbing
Get up the tree; sitting on
branch; looking at view
Seeing many different trees
Getting on top of a branch
A “step” upwards
The bending of the
branches
The shape of the branches
The green color of leaves
The brightness of reflected
sunlight from the tree
This table is organized with the higher levels at the top, but it is best read from the bottom
upwards because each higher level perception is formed from the perceptual levels below.
The hierarchy is branched rather than linear, but for ease of explanation, just one example
is provided at each level.
it also provides a framework for computational modeling of
psychological pathways (Mansell and Huddy, 2020). Notably,
PCT has influenced the development of theories used within the
nature engagement field such as choice theory (Glasser, 1981)
and wild system theory (Jordan, 2008). PCT also has an evidence
base across the disciplines most relevant to nature engagement,
including ethology (Pellis and Bell, 2020), neuroscience (Yin,
2017), mental health (Alsawy et al., 2014), and sociology
(McClelland, 2020).
Whilst an advanced quantitative research design might model
and test potential pathways from nature to well-being specified
by PCT in the future, an empirical bridge can be provided
by qualitative research. Within open self-reports, people who
perceive improvements in well-being from engaging with nature
can, within the limits of introspection, provide multiple themes
regarding the mechanisms they notice as important.
Examples of the most relevant qualitative research studies that
have explored the pathways from nature engagement to well-
being are summarized in Table 2. There are many more examples
of studies that have focused on specific activities, such as various
hobbies and sports, which are not summarized here.
The qualitative studies to date have echoed the findings
within the quantitative research, but this small number of
TABLE 2 | Examples of earlier qualitative studies and their findings regarding the
pathways between nature engagement and well-being.
References Methods
Key findings
Windhorst and 12 students prompted to Participants chose environments
Williams (2015) photograph the elements of that were familiar, an escape, and
their natural environment enabled self-reflection
that they felt contributed to
their well-being and mental
health
Martyn and
Brymer (2016)
305 students. Analysis of
text-based answers to the
question of “what being in
nature means to you”
Seven themes: sensory
engagement, expanse, connection
(including social and nature
connectedness), time out,
relaxation, enjoyment (including
feelings of restoration), and a
healthy perspective
McCree et al.
(2018)
11 disadvantaged children
given highly interactive,
child-centered interviews
A meta-theme of “establishing
self-regulation and resilience
through emotional space.”
Additional themes of autonomy,
freedom, meeting basic needs,
engaging in physically adventurous
activity, free social play, discovering
about nature, becoming socially
confident learners, making choices,
and gaining independence
Coventry et al. 45 adults engaging in
(2019)
conservation activities in
public green spaces
Degree of personal meaningfulness
and control over the specific activity
related to a sense of satisfaction
Birch et al.
(2020)
24 young multi-ethnic urban Three themes of relational felt sense
residents
within nature were described:
self-acceptance, escape, and
connection and care
Brymer et al.
(2021)
15 people who reported
enhanced well-being from
leisure activities in nature
Three overarching themes were “a
sense of perspective,” “mental and
emotional sanctuary,” and “being
immersed in the moment,”
supporting an interactive
relationship between people and
the natural environment
studies has also expanded the scope of mechanisms at
multiple levels of experience (sensory, attentional, self-concept,
etc), and across intrapersonal, interpersonal and systemic
(e.g., school) domains. The most prominent strand running
through them appeared to be how natural environments
provide a spectrum of opportunities for people of all ages
to meet their multiple biological and psychological needs:
independence/autonomy/control, physical activity, emotional
expression and problem resolution, play, discovery, personal
growth, self-reflection, self-acceptance, and social connectedness.
Given the limited number of studies, but their wide scope and
limited integration to date, we designed an interview study that
aimed to identify and organize the potential pathways within an
interdisciplinary framework.
In order to meet the above aim, we required a methodology
that met the following criteria: (1) a sample who identified
that nature engagement activities had benefited their well-being
and/or mental health and were therefore motivated to talk in
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org
4
March 2021 | Volume 12 | Article 580992
Iqbal and Mansell
Nature Engagement Pathways
detail about potential mechanisms; (2) a convenient sample
that were articulate, available to devote the time, and able to
reflect on the benefits that nature had during their lives—
therefore undergraduate university students were recruited; (3)
a semi-structured interview that focused on identifying potential
pathways that emerged from the participants themselves, rather
than specific questions regarding specific pathways. We aimed
to compare our findings with the wider empirical literature to
interpret them within the context of theories of the benefits
of nature engagement and the nature of subjective, social and
psychological well-being. In order to do so, we adopted an
inductive approach, guided by the idiosyncratic experiences of
the clients. Following this we examined existing gaps in theory
and explored whether PCT might help integrate the pathways
within an analytical model, specific to the findings of the
current study.
bottom-up approach to identify themes. The first step the first
author took in generating themes was by familiarizing and
understanding the data set. The first author read the transcripts
multiple times to get to know the data set and to think if there are
any common answers or patterns thought all the interviews. Then
the first author started to code the transcripts, which allowed
her to develop an understanding of the data set further. The
first author then grouped the common codes into meaningful
categories, narrowing the choices for themes. The next step was to
make potential themes, which resulted in 11 themes. Finally, the
second author reviewed the themes and their associated quotes.
After discussion, it was decided that the previous names did not
accurately capture the essence of each theme and so these were
updated and finalized.
RESULTS AND DISCUSSION
METHODS
Design
This study used qualitative research methods, through a semi-
structured interview.
Participants
The study was advertised through an email from the university
communication email service, requesting for undergraduate
students who have experiences with nature engagement in their
childhood and adolescence, and who are willing to talk about the
role of experience with nature in their mental health and well-
being. Table 3 shows the participants demographics and what
nature-related activities they engaged in.
Materials
The topic guide was developed by the first author, reviewed
and discussed with the second author, and piloted with two
individuals reporting benefits of nature engagement before being
refined as the study version (Table 4), The interviews were
recorded using an encrypted audio recorder.
Procedure
After establishing email contact, the participants were emailed
an information sheet about the study, explaining the aims of
the study and what was required of them. The interviews took
place in a testing cubicle in the Psychology department of the
university. When the participant arrived at the interview, they
were asked to complete a consent form and a short demographic
form. The semi-structured interviews was conducted, the
participants were thanked for taking part in the study and they
had the opportunity to ask any questions.
All the interviews were transcribed. After the recordings were
fully transcribed, each audio recording was deleted from the
encrypted file. The first author carried out a thematic analysis
using Braun and Clarke’s guidelines (Braun and Clarke, 2006).
Thematic analysis is a technique to categorize themes and
patterns across a dataset about the research question (Braun
and Clarke, 2013). Thematic analysis is flexible, allowing the
researcher to answer a research question with ease. It uses a
After applying thematic analysis to the transcriptions, 11 themes
emerged from the data set. These will be explained in a
hierarchical order from sensory experiences upwards, similar to
the vertical dimension of Table 1 and Figure 1, culminating in
the themes that illustrate the apparent overall function of nature
engagement in participants’ lives. Each theme is discussed in
relation to the wider theory and evidence, and a complementary,
integrative role of PCT is introduced.
Theme 1: Enjoy the Different Sensory
Inputs
This theme is about how the participants enjoyed the sensory
input they received from nature, such as visual, auditory, tactile,
and olfactory. The sensory input gave the participants a pleasing
sensory experience. The participants explained how they highly
appreciated different sensory contributions. Some participants
explained that they valued a broad horizon and the different
colors from the natural environment, other valued the quietness
and the sounds that came from natural sources such as trees, or
the smell of nature, or even the feeling of grass. Polly said:
“When I go home, I just feel so much better and as much as
I enjoy being at uni and it’s a whole new different lease of
life, going home, seeing the stars again because there’s no light
pollution, lying on the ground feeling the grass.”
This demonstrated that the participants liked the simple effects
that nature gave them, possibly due to the minimal effort
for mental processing. It was easy for them to take in the
different sensory experiences; it does not involve feelings of stress
or anxiety. In the natural environment, there are no proper
requirements. Therefore, it enabled individuals to take in the
different modes of sensory input. It could be that an individual’s
subconscious allowed them to experience how peaceful and quiet
nature is. Harper said:
“I’d say on a concrete level, literally the fresh air, like when I
breathe in, I think it tastes different. . . the feeling the fresh air
and seeing and I feel like there are a lot of colors are going
on with it and you get the smells of flowers and plants and its
quite sensory.”
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org
5
March 2021 | Volume 12 | Article 580992
Iqbal and Mansell
Nature Engagement Pathways
TABLE 3 | Participants’ characteristics.
Pseudonyms
Age range
Gender
Marian
Polly
18–25
F
18–25
F
Liz
Anne
Harper
Nina
Aimen
40–50
F
18–25
F
18–25
F
18–25
F
18–25
F
Ethnicity
Italian/Romanian
White British
White British
Belgian
White British
Indian
Pakistani
Nature-related activities engaged with
Climbing, walking dogs, pet therapy, playing with animals
Hiking, scouts (hiking, camping, fires, learning about nature), gardening, tree planting,
walks, outdoor yoga, bug hunts, painting/art, canoeing, swimming, feeding animals,
cleaning animals
Walking in the countryside, farming/harvesting, caring for animals, swimming in the
sea, playing on the beach, surfing
Hiking, walking, running, climbing
Forest walks, playing in the park, looking for birds, animal engagement, feeding
animals
Walking, nature-based craft activities
Gardening, walking, playing with sand, farming
TABLE 4 | Topic guide for the semi-structured interview questions.
Please could you start by telling me about the nature-related activities you
liked as a child. Which of these activities do you still engage in?
What was it about these activities that you liked?
How would you say that these activities impacted your well-being or mental
health as a child? Why do you think it impacted you in this way? Does it still
affect you?
Can you describe an example of when these nature-related activities
benefited your well-being? How do you think it helped?
If you were not able to engage in these activities, how do you think it would
affect your well-being? Was there a time when you weren’t able to do them?
How did it make you feel? Why?
Do you think that any of these activities has affected you as an individual?
now? If so, could you explain to me how you think it has had this effect on
you?
Is there anything else you would like to cover that I may have missed?
Do have any question you would like to ask?
Participants described how nature allowed them to explore the
scene, and there were diverse experiences they could get from
their environment. As Harper explained, the sensory input is on
a concrete level. Therefore, the eyes, ears, nose, and hands have
to a specific degree, evidence, that their senses were fulfilled with
what they subconsciously enjoy. Participant reported the natural
sounds such as rain, leaves rustling, and the sea that had a calming
effect, and described how that this effect brought joy. Similarly,
with the other sensory inputs, they had other positive effects, for
e.g., smelling flowers and freshly cut grass, or looking at a vast
horizon was aesthetically pleasing to the eye, which in turn led to
participants’ reports of enjoyment.
The increase in positive affect and the reduction of unpleasant
affect encapsulate a facet of subjective well-being (Diener et al.,
1999), and the role of sensory input from nature in affect
regulation nature has been a key theme of earlier research (e.g.,
Ulrich, 1979; Martyn and Brymer, 2016). Indeed, the theme of
“immersion in the present moment” in a recent qualitative study
described the role of the richness of perceptual experience within
a natural environment that facilitates absorption (Brymer et al.,
2021). Moreover, the engagement with natural beauty has been
found to mediate the relationship between nature connectedness
and happiness (Richardson and McEwan, 2018). As mentioned in
the Introduction, the biophilia hypothesis explains these sensory
preferences for nature as evolutionary in origin (Wilson, 1984),
their facilitation of evolved affective systems are described in
SRT (Ulrich, 1983) and these are operationalised as goal-directed
sensory the preferences within ART (Kaplan, 1995). These
theories make the fair assumption that humans have sensory
preferences, and PCT has the grounding within engineering
and mathematics to describe the working mechanism in detail.
According to PCT, the behavior of humans and other animals
is the manifestation of controlled sensory input and this can be
modeled as a negative feedback control system (Powers, 1973).
Taken together, our participants’ expression of the enjoyment
of sensory input from nature may provide neurophysiological
balance precisely because it permits them to act, freely, to control
the input to their senses as they observe and navigate through a
natural environment.
Theme 2: Feel Freedom
This theme is about how nature-related activities made people
feel as if they were free and liberated, often because they did not
need to conform with the expectations of others. Marian said:
“You can get to be yourself because no one is judging you and
is looking at you, like animals don’t care about the way you
look or whatever you’re doing, they’re just there with you. And
you don’t have to force it.”
The participants described feeling free to roam around and
explore, and they seemed to link this to some of the other benefits
of nature. For example, Nina said:
“I would feel a lot more free and you can separate yourself from
the things that are stressing you and allows to problem solve.”
The autonomy facet of psychological well-being (Ryff and Keyes,
1995) comes closest to the feeling of freedom theme described
here, and it has emerged in other qualitative studies (e.g.,
Martyn and Brymer, 2016; McCree et al., 2018). Given the
subjective nature of this theme, it is hard to assess objectively,
yet there is a small but robust benefit in attentional control—the
ability to orient and sustain attention of one’s own volition—
measured after nature engagement (Stevenson et al., 2018). The
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org
6
March 2021 | Volume 12 | Article 580992
Iqbal and Mansell
Nature Engagement Pathways
feeling of freedom is implicit within theoretical accounts of
nature engagement, such as the goal compatibility feature of
ART describes the benefits of experiences that are self-selected,
also a component of self-regulation theory (Korpela, 1995) and
self-determination theory (Deci and Ryan, 1985). PCT echoes
this self-regulatory function; the intrinsic need to control one’s
sensory experiences through freedom of action is at the heart of
PCT, guiding psychological development throughout the lifespan
(Plooij, 2020). Method of Levels, a therapy based on PCT,
provides clients with the freedom to control their own access to
therapy and the choice of problem to discuss, and the therapist
uses questioning to facilitate the client’s ability to shift and sustain
attention to various facets of the experiences they choose to
discuss (Carey, 2006; Mansell, 2018).
Theme 3: Calm Nature Facilitates a Calm
Mood
Participants explained that nature is calm in terms of sounds and
the overall atmosphere; it influenced the individual’s feelings. The
atmosphere corresponded to their emotions. The participants
described various specific examples of how experiences of nature
enabled their calm and relaxed mood. Marian said:
“It can change your mood, definitely. Like the calmness of the
trees helps you to be calm as well.”
Similarly, Nina said:
“The feeling of breeze and fresh air on your skin and the
sounds in nature are softer so trees rustling and animals and
those more gentle sounds. So they are very calming for me.
And also the feeling of grass, laying on the grass.”
In each case, there was a confluence between the gentle nature
of the experiences of nature and the calming, relaxed mood the
participants experienced.
The feeling of calmness and relaxation form a component of
subjective well-being (Diener et al., 1999). The capacity for the
aesthetic qualities of natural scenes to faciliate calm mood forms
one of the affective reactions described in SRT (Ulrich, 1983).
More recently, Richardson’s emotion regulation theory describes
a neurophysiological pathway for this effect in substantial detail,
via the balancing of the parasympathetic (PNS) and sympathetic
(SNS) nervous systems (Richardson, 2019) and it builds upon
research indicating that the majority of individuals experience
an increase in PNS and a decrease in SNS activation when
engaged with nature [reviewed by Richardson (2019)]. There is
convergent evidence that the PNS sustains a reflective mindset
that in itself is an aid to well-being via its impact on self-
reflection, perspective-taking and decision-making (Kok and
Fredrickson, 2010); this links the current theme with additional
themes extracted in the study, as discussed later. Yet, it is
important to recognize that individuals in certain circumstances
experience a perceived danger within natural environments, and
this may be experienced as undesirable (e.g., animal phobias) or
even as desirable (e.g., in outdoor risk-taking pursuits; Lupton
and Tulloch, 2002). Thus, the potential for nature engagement
to be calming may be one, albeit important, function regarding
emotion regulation (Richardson, 2019). The role of “calming”
mood within a PCT framework is described as it emerges in
later themes.
Theme 4: Enhancing Decision Making and
Forming Action Plans
This theme refers to how nature-related activities allowed
individuals to make a concrete action plan and make decisions.
The participants explained how they found it easier to make plans
when they engaged with nature relative to when they were inside.
Anne said:
“I’m working and I’m inside, all these different thoughts pop
up and I can get a bit disorganized and then when I’m outside I
think it’s much easier to organize my thoughts a little bit more
and come up with an action plan. And then really come back
inside and get back to what I need to do. . . it allows me to see
the different things that I need to do and organize them.”
Engaging with nature helped the participants to clear their mind.
It helped to stop thinking about unnecessary thoughts, that was
previously on the participant’s mind, which led to more excessive
unhelpful thinking patterns, and enabled them to contemplate on
the essential factors. For example, Liz said:
“A platform to solve problems. . . if I take myself out of a
situation and I can look at it more objectively and that’s what
nature gives to me... To overcome the panic that instilled and
with that clarity, you can start to put plans in place to work
with the challenge.”
This theme clearly reflects the pathway to well-being described
within ART and in particular the importance of “being away”
to escape from distraction, reduce urgency, and engage a more
reflective mode of thought (Kaplan, 1995). Decision-making and
action planning are mental processes that are also integral to self-
regulation (Korpela, 1995). The benefit of natural environments
for reflective processing have been identified in earlier qualitative
studies (e.g., Windhorst and Williams, 2015). Indeed recent
years have seen an emergence of studies finding improvement
in sustained attention (Pasanen T. et al., 2018) and creative
problem-solving after periods of immersion in nature (Atchley
et al., 2012; Ferraro, 2015) and there is evidence from structural
equation models that the attentional focus facilitated by the
natural environment contributes to these benefits (Pasanen T.
P. et al., 2018). Our participants did not specifically describe
creativity as a benefit of nature engagement, but they did describe
the capacity to contemplate their priorities and solve problems.
On the face of it, the social, subjective and psychological
perspectives on well-being do not describe the capacity to make
decisions and action plans as a facet of well-being. Nonetheless,
they might be expected to contribute to autonomy and personal
growth (Ryff and Keyes, 1995). PCT may complement the above
approaches by specifying in more detail the state required for
decision-making; it is a neurophysiological state that allows
allows conflicted goals to be held in awareness to explore the
potential decisions for long enough, and in sufficient detail, to
come to a solution through reorganization (Mansell, 2020).
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org
7
March 2021 | Volume 12 | Article 580992
Iqbal and Mansell
Nature Engagement Pathways
Theme 5: Enhancement of Efficiency and
Productivity
Participants explained that after they engaged with nature-
related activities, they were much more productive and efficient
in their work, relative to before engaging with the activity.
Nina said:
“I am much more productive if I take out some time to go out.”
Participants described engaging with nature as a positive
distraction from working because when the individual
was working, they focused on one thing for a significant
amount of time such as an essay, thus when the individual
engaged with nature they had many different things
they could divert their attention to for example the sky,
the horizon, or an animal. This made the individual feel
revitalized, which enabled them to become productive later.
Aimen said:
“During exams you say you’re going to revise from this time
to this time, so you need a break, best thing you can do it that
break, is to go spend some time with the natural environment,
in nature, with fresh air. It freshens your mind, it gives you
more energy, it motivates you, like okay I should go back to
what I was doing.”
Participants reported that when they studied but could not
focus, and they decided to engage with nature, after the
activity, they noticed that their efficiency and productivity
levels had increased, allowing them to study more than
before they had engaged with nature. Some used it as
an effective strategy to use for a break; they can exert
some energy, feeling refreshed, as Aimen said “it freshens
your mind.”
This theme appears to represent an overarching benefit from
the enhanced decision-making and restoration provided by
nature, and maps onto aspects of subjective well-being such as the
pleasant feeling of energy and refreshment, and work satisfaction
(Diener et al., 1999). To some degree, it appeared to also feed
into the sense of environmental mastery and autonomy, facets of
psychological well-being (Ryff and Keyes, 1995), that participants
experienced. Maybe because this theme represents the tail-end
of the pathway from nature to well-being, it is not captured as
a component of nature engagement theories, but it would be
consistent as an outcome of them, especially ART (Kaplan, 1995)
and self-regulation theory (Korpela, 1995). There is emerging
evidence, building the pathway from creative problem-solving,
that dispositional nature engagement is associated with a more
innovative cognitive style, which is regarded as a benefit for
educational and commercial organizations (Leong et al., 2014).
From the perspective of PCT, “productivity” and “efficiency”
represent principles within a perceptual hierarchy. Immediately
above the level of principles are system concepts, including
concepts of the self, others, organizations and the wider world.
Thus, to the extent that an individual pursues and upholds the
principles of efficiency and productivity within their self-concept,
they will experience benefits in well-being from the properties of
nature engagement that have these effects.
Theme 6: Alleviating Pressure From
Society’s Expectations Regarding
Education
Many participants explained how nature-related activities helped
them through the difficult education system. The demands
of education appeared to be profound in the case of some
participants, but the amount of stress varied depending on the
individual. Liz said:
“I’m in the cohort of the first people to do GCSE’s. . . the
pressure on us was extraordinary. . . so that 2 years, that 14–
16 years was very stressful and I identified a walk that I would
go on. . . because of having that degree of connection, reduced
my stress. . . in a way it continues now, I think at that point
things felt like they were going very fast and in nature things
don’t go fast and they go very slow. . . . So, it was certainly that
morning walk that set me on course to maintain my mental
health during school day, it sort of gave me that reflection and
preparation time.”
The participants described how the positive effects described
within earlier themes helped to lessen the pressure of education.
When the individual stepped out of a place that was causing them
anxiety and which created a great deal of pressure, nature enabled
them not to become overwhelmed. Nina said:
“I was doing my GCSE exams, so when I was about 15, the
I would go take breaks at the weekend and go outside for a
long walk and because it was a stressful time and I felt very
confined when I was inside surrounded by work and they were
the biggest exams I had done at that stage in my life, going
outside would calm me down and I would feel a lot more free
and you can separate yourself from the things that are stressing
you and allows to problem solve.”
When the participants got the opportunity to go out, they felt as
if they could do things at their time, there was a sense of ease and
control. This contrasted with how they described the education
system, where control is taken away from students because they
are told what to so and when to do it.
This theme may have been specific to the current sample
because they were young adults reflecting on their life, which
was largely dominated by academic work. Within the domain of
employment there is emerging evidence that nature engagement
breaks reduce job stress (de Bloom et al., 2017) and there is also
evidence that nature engagement in schools leads to improved
engagement with classroom education in primary school children
(Kuo et al., 2018). The theme involved participants experiencing
a break from the pressure of societal expectations regarding
education, and the experience of a lack of control over such
requirements. This theme therefore points to the importance
of understanding multiple levels of experience—including the
system concept of society’s expectations—which an individual
can choose to conform to, or not, at any moment in time. Thus,
the facets of psychological well-being such as autonomy and
purpose in life may benefit via this pathway. The theme also
dovetails with the focus of PCT of the importance of control in
well-being, and the necessity to shift awareness to higher levels of
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org
8
March 2021 | Volume 12 | Article 580992
Iqbal and Mansell
Nature Engagement Pathways
perception to achieve change through a shift in perspective. This
links the current theme to later themes of both feeling freedom
and nature putting life into perspective.
Theme 7: Formation of Community
Relations
This theme is about how individuals formed community
relationships through nature engagement activities. The activities
were a way toward a strong bond with one another. Polly said:
“Cause I’ve got a real community there it is obvious that like
it’s a supplement for my well-being so if I don’t have time
for it then it doesn’t help with the stressful situations at all. . .
scouting is just, has completely shaped my life, the skills that
you get there and the people that you meet.”
Not only were these activities a way to build relationships, but
these relationships helped to deal with stressful life events that
occurred. These types of relationships last a very long time
because there is a strong foundation of how the relationship
formed, through the mutual enjoyment of a particular activity.
Marian said:
“We were out with all the people, we didn’t spend time inside,
like playing video games or watching television, we literally
had to go outside. . . we kind of like created a team.”
Participants reported that interacting with others improved their
mood, and they helped to form tight-knit communities because
they could relate with each other through a shared interest.
This theme describes an element of social well-being,
especially the sense of connectedness and social support (Larson,
1993). It is also a facet of subjective well-being as satisfaction
with one’s social group (Diener et al., 1999), and psychological
well-being as positive relations (Ryff and Keyes, 1995). The
community relations pathway has been noted as a theme
in earlier qualitative research (O’Brien et al., 2010; Genter
et al., 2015), yet theories of nature engagement have tended
to focus on connectedness with nature itself (Bragg, 1996) and
spiritual connectedness (Ferrer, 2002), and their interrelationship
(Kamitsis and Francis, 2013). Nonetheless, there is evidence
that even a 2-week nature-based intervention can promote pro-
social orientation and connectedness to other people (Passmore
and Holder, 2017), thus potentially improving social well-being
and helping to draw upon others as a source of support. From
the perspective of PCT, communities are system concepts—the
highest level of perception in PCT—and so being part of a
community may be fundamental to well-being for many people.
Theme 8: Nature Puts Things Into
Perspective
This theme is about how nature-related activities allowed the
participants to improve their viewpoint on a negative situation
and enabled them to realize the severity of a situation and how
significant, or insignificant, it truly was. Through the change
in thought process, they felt better after engaging with nature-
related activities. Liz said:
“It’s about putting things into perspective quality it seems to
have. . . a period when myself and my other half were looking
for a house and we were spending most weekends driving
around houses, so we weren’t doing our normal leisure stuff at
the weekend and moving houses is stressful anyway and I felt
my enjoyment in it going down and my frustration in it going
up. . . let’s go to the peak district instead and using it again to get
things into perspective, get them to a manageable state. . . I’d
sort of thing well the trees don’t care; the river doesn’t care. It
would help me retain perspective, having that calm beginning
to the day.”
Engaging with nature allowed the individual to have a
connection, by appreciating how amazing nature is and how
everything is meant to be in life, and a link was made by putting
the negative situation into perspective. For some, it enabled the
individual to believe that things would fall into place, giving a
sense of hope. Marian said:
“I mean you feel like your problems are so small compared
to like how big the forest is like the view from the mountain
and everything.”
A comparison was made with nature to the problem, realizing
that it was not as significant as they previously thought. The
participants tended to treat the problem as an animate object
when comparing it to the natural environment. For example,
Marian said “your problems are so small compared to like how
big the forest is.” This comparison was then used to help the
individual understand that they could manage their hardships.
This theme appeared to illustrate how nature could help
people re-establish their purpose in life, a facet of psychological
well-being (Ryff and Keyes, 1995). The capacity for nature to
provide a beneficial higher level perspective on one’s life struggles
is a feature of a number of theories, including the spirituality
of transpersonal theory (Ferrer, 2002), the perspective of an
ecological self (Bragg, 1996), and the role of the awe experienced
through witnessing natural environments as described within
ART (Kaplan, 1995). This pathway was elucidated by a recent
qualitative study of leisure engagement in nature that described
the theme of “a sense of perspective” to include a sense of
oneness with nature, and feelings of humility and gratitude in
the face of the awe of nature. The perspective provided by
nature also receives support from studies indicating that nature
connectedness mediates the benefits of nature (Kamitsis and
Francis, 2013; Trigwell et al., 2014). Interestingly, the sustained
awareness of a higher level perspective on one’s conflicting
goals at any level, is the mechanism of recovery from the
distress experienced by chronic goal conflict as described by PCT
(Powers, 1973); thus the perspective-widening capacity of nature
may be another fundamental pathway to well-being.
Theme 9: Liking the Contrast From the
Urban Environment
Participants made a clear contrast between the natural
environment and the urban environment. They reported
that the urban environment can be hectic, where an individual
needs to watch out for cars, bikes and general traffic, whereas
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org
9
March 2021 | Volume 12 | Article 580992
Iqbal and Mansell
Nature Engagement Pathways
when in nature there is no need to be as cautious because such
things are not an issue. Liz said:
“Leaving behind man-made noise and entering an
environment where there is only natural noise has a
distinct neurological effect on you. It seems to calm you and
make you more alert at the same time.”
The participants reported that the natural environment brings
about a sense of peacefulness, possibly because there is not much
pressure on cognitive functioning; for example, Liz said “seems to
calm you.” Participants reported that in the urban environment,
people have to pay attention to their surroundings to look out for
danger, even for simple tasks such as changing buses in the city
center. Harper said:
“I feel like when you live in the city, when the cars zoom past it
subconsciously causes a bit of anxiety in you, whereas in nature
the calmness and stillness, like the contrast between the city
and natural environment, you feel less anxious subconsciously
because of the difference.”
The contrasting features of the urban and natural environment
are integral to the many theories of nature benefits and implicit
within qualitative studies. The reasons for this contrast relates to
the various nature engagement pathways (e.g., the contrasting
sensory input). Nonetheless, we extracted this theme because
the participants overtly stated and elaborated upon the contrast.
Indeed there is evidence within research on self-regulation
theory that people hold “place identities” for natural locations—
places where they feel an emotional bond or strong sense of
attachment (Ratcliffe and Korpela, 2018) and these may be
the natural locations that they realize meet their psychological
needs (Landon et al., 2020). Within PCT, this choice of current
surroundings would involve programs (e.g., routines, plans such
as hikes, fishing trips, forest school, etc) that are situated in the
middle of a goal hierarchy between the higher levels of principles
and system concepts, and the lower levels that control more
immediate experiences such as the choice of landscapes, plants
and animals to attend to. Controlling programs of this kind
would enable them to experience the desired qualities of natural
environment over the urban environment.
Theme 10: Coping Mechanism
Many of the participants explicitly used nature as a coping
mechanism. If they were stressed out or going through a difficult
time, they engaged in nature-related activities, which helped
them cope with their emotion. Polly said:
“I just have to go in nature to feel a bit better. So I can literally
go lie on the ground and look at the sky and even though it
won’t solve the problem, it will temporarily make me feel a bit
better, so I can go back practically deal with the situation.”
This theme drew upon the earlier themes of sensory input
as enjoyable, and nature as calming, because the process of
coping drew upon these qualities of nature-related activities. For
example, participants described how they would seek out nature
when stressed and it would help them to relax. For example,
in some cases, attention was diverted to something aesthetically
pleasing such as stars in the sky; for example, Polly said “go in
nature to feel a bit better.” Therefore, being around a pleasing
aspect of nature enabled the individual to place their attention
to something enjoyable to look at, thus resulted in positive
psychological changes in the body. Anne said:
“I get really stressed, so I’ll go outside and go for a walk for
a few minutes. . . So being able to go outside and clear my
head really helps. . . It’s both a coping mechanism and also
preventative because I know that if I don’t do it influence me.”
Nature engagement as coping is a theme within earlier studies
(e.g., Chawla, 2020). Coping has some overlap with the facets
of psychological well-being such as autonomy and the concept
of environmental mastery (Ryff and Keyes, 1995). It is also a
key process within self-regulatory theories of nature engagement
(Korpela, 1995), and “coping” as a construct may link together
the fields of ART, emotion regulation and self-regulation, in that
the choice to engage with nature to cope with stress may enable
restore the balance of the PNS and SNS systems, and through
its impact on attentional processes, also restore concentration
(Berto, 2014). PCT may be able to integrate the themes described
earlier to specify a specific pathway for long-term coping–the
reduction of chronic unresolved goal conflict (Powers, 1973). It
pinpoints the reason for a misbalance in physiological systems
as chronic suppression of “action-ready” bodily systems that
are thwarted by conflicting goals (Mansell, 2020). Therefore,
long-term coping emerges when people can select the kinds of
environments that allow them to engage with and reflect upon
their goal conflicts and resolve them through reorganization.
Reduction in arousal and improvement in concentration then
may emerge as the conflict is reduced. It is possible that this
property of natural environments leads them to be considered
by some as a “mental and emotional sanctuary” (Brymer et al.,
2021).
Theme 11: Anxious if Prevented or
Restricted
Participants explained how if the participants were prevented
from engaging in nature-related activities, they would become
anxious. Liz said:
“I’d notice anxiety going up, yeah. And it could creep up on
you, if you spend a couple of months and for some reason
you’ve not managed to do it and haven’t managed to get out
of town, I just find myself more worried about everything.”
The participants were aware that because their nature
engagement activities were used as a stress release, when
their means to release stress was taken away, anxiety increased.
Harper said:
“I feel like I would get antsy. . . I would be more stressed out
and like anxious and I just wouldn’t be enjoying it.”
It is a logical conclusion that anxiety and stress may resurface
when access to nature is limited, given its benefits for stress
reduction. Nonetheless, the participants’ were particularly aware
of this dependence on nature in their accounts and its effect on
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org
10
March 2021 | Volume 12 | Article 580992
Iqbal and Mansell
Nature Engagement Pathways
FIGURE 2 | A diagram of the themes in the study, organized to illustrate the multi-level pathways through which nature engagement as a coping mechanism leads to
multi-level benefits to well-being. The themes outside the box are worded as in the Results and Discussion. However, within the box, the rest of the themes have been
organized hierarchically, and the Urban vs. Nature theme has been used to contrast each of these perceptions between the two environments using material from
participants’ accounts.
their subjective well-being (Diener et al., 1999). Their experience
could be interpreted as a validation of the view that access to
nature is a basic need owing to our biological heritage (Wilson,
1984). Alternatively, it could be claimed that using a “dose”
of nature to “cope” is insufficient, in a similar way to the
resurgence of anxiety during withdrawal from medication for
emotional disorders. A potential integrative stance is that nature
engagement has a unique and intrinsic capacity to combine
multiple pathways to well-being, yet it is not always sufficient
in facilitating benefits that sustain. Future research could explore
whether certain forms of nature engagement, possibly those that
facilitate higher-level perspectives on conflicting aspects of the
self, as described in PCT, have long-term benefits.
GENERAL DISCUSSION
The combination of themes in the current study is novel, and
also somewhat comprehensive, seeming to encompass many of
the themes from earlier qualitative studies, many of which also
have an empirical basis within quantitative research. Therefore,
we attempt to fully integrate them here (see Figure 2).
The bottom line inside the box in Figure 2 describes the lower
levels of perceptual experiences within the natural environment.
These levels of perception would include intensities, sensations,
configurations and transitions within the PCT hierarchy. The
specific aesthetic qualities of these perceptions within the natural
environment and how they facilitate adaptive neurophysiological
modes of action are well-described by SRT, ART and Emotion
Regulation Theory. Key examples of their beneficial pathways
emerging from the analysis are a calm mood (a valued outcome
in Figure 2), and a free exploration of experience (going up a
level in Figure 2), which in turn enables creative problem-solving
(a further valued outcome in Figure 2) which in turn leads
to further valued outcomes including improved efficiency and
productivity (Figure 2). Interestingly, the perceptual hierarchy
in PCT can short-circuit the exterm environment and enter an
imagination mode whereby stored perceptions are experienced
“as if ” real to the brain (Powers, 1973). This capacity has been
used to explain the role of imagery in psychological interventions
(e.g., Mansell and Hodson, 2009), and there is evidence that
mental imagery of natural scenes may be particularly beneficial,
for example in reducing state anxiety (Nguyen and Brymer,
2018).
As well as providing a hierarchical framework, PCT describes
the nature of a “problem” and how it is solved. A person is primed
to act on a goal, entailing physiological arousal, yet becomes stuck
with these feelings, which are now experienced as stress owing
to thwarted goal progress, and this requires a creative solution
(Mansell, 2020). PCT proposes that the solution will emerge if
attention is directed and sustained above the systems in conflict
until the process of reorganization allows spontaneous, trial-and-
error changes that resolve it. It is notable therefore that there
are higher levels of perception in Figure 2—connection with a
community and a wider view on one’s life from the perspective
of one’s connection with the natural world. In order to resolve
issues relating to one’s own conflicting standards or social roles,
PCT would indicate that a person needs to shift and sustain their
awareness above these levels to eventually resolve them, and one
of these resolutions includes alleviation from the pressures of the
education system (the final valued outcome in Figure 2). The
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org
11
March 2021 | Volume 12 | Article 580992
Iqbal and Mansell
Nature Engagement Pathways
additional themes in Figure 2 are outside the box because they
indicate that, owing to the above benefits, natural environments
themselves are a controlled perception, to be used as a coping
strategy (a “program” in PCT terms), and anxiety is experienced
when this control of environment is restricted or not possible.
Importantly, this model is analytical rather than formal, and
the aim of future research will be to formalize these components
within a PCT architecture and construct individualized models
that can be tested for their fit with real world data within
natural environments. This methodology is challenging, but it
provides a particularly robust test of a psychological theory
(Mansell and Huddy, 2020). The functional specification of a
model will also facilitate the research and translational impact
within new technology, such as smartphones and augmented or
virtual reality, which is a burgeoning area in nature engagement
research (Wooller et al., 2018; MacIntyre et al., 2020; Yin
et al., 2020). Arguably, the closest conceptualization to date
to encompass the multi-layered benefits of nature engagement
is the phenomenological-psychoanalytic, ecological-existential
framework of Brymer and colleagues (e.g., Immonen et al., 2018;
Schweitzer et al., 2018). One clear distinction is that perceptual
control theory provides a single theoretical framework, in
contrast to a pluralistic integration of multiple theories.
There were a number of limitations of this study. First,
although the sample were of the desired age range and
showed some ethnic diversity, only university-educated female
participants were recruited. Therefore, different themes may have
emerged with a larger sample of a wider range of education and
a representative balance of genders. Second, the semi-structured
interview format was retrospective. This was an advantage in that
the research captured a historic overview of the most personally
meaningful aspects of nature. However, it relied on memory and
focused on general themes rather than idiosyncratic, dynamic
experiences. In future, using distributed ethnographic models
with participants self-coding would increase the granularity of
the findings and provide mode detail on the context of the
findings for future research.
Overall, this study has replicated and extended the themes
from earlier qualitative studies exploring the pathways of benefit
from nature engagement. These themes have been parsed into a
multiple levels and analyzed in the context of constructs of well-
being, a range of theories of nature engagement benefits, and
perceptual control theory to generate an integrative account to
guide future research and practice.
DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be
made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
ETHICS STATEMENT
The studies involving human participants were reviewed
and approved by UREC, University of Manchester. The
patients/participants provided their written informed consent to
participate in this study.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
WM conceived of the study and co-designed it. AI ran
the study and the data analysis. Both authors wrote
the manuscript.
REFERENCES
Allan, J., Hardwell, A., Kay, C., Peacock, S., Hart, M., Dillon, M., et al. (2020).
Health and wellbeing in an outdoor and adventure sports context. Sports 8:50.
doi: 10.3390/sports8040050
Alsawy, S., Mansell, W., Carey, T. A., McEvoy, P., and Tai, S. J. (2014). Science and
practice of transdiagnostic CBT: a perceptual control theory (PCT) approach.
Int. J. Cogn. Ther. 7, 334–359. doi: 10.1521/ijct.2014.7.4.334
Araújo, D., Brymer, E., Brito, H., Withagen, R., and Davids, K. (2019).
The empowering variability of affordances of nature: why do exercisers
feel better after performing the same exercise in natural environments
than in indoor environments? Psychol. Sport Exerc. 42, 138–145.
doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2018.12.020
Atchley, R. A., Strayer, D. L., and Atchley, P. (2012). Creativity in the wild:
Improving creative reasoning through immersion in natural settings. PLoS
ONE 7:e51474. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0051474
Barton, J., and Pretty, J. (2010). What is the best dose of nature and green exercise
for improving mental health? A multi-study analysis. Environ. Sci. Technol. 44,
3947–3955. doi: 10.1021/es903183r
Berto, R. (2014). The role of nature in coping with psycho-physiological
stress: a literature review on restorativeness. Behav. Sci. 4, 394–409.
doi: 10.3390/bs4040394
Birch, J., Rishbeth, C., and Payne, S. R. (2020). Nature doesn’t judge you–how
urban nature supports young people’s mental health and wellbeing in a diverse
UK city. Health Place 62:102296. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2020.102296
Bragg, E. A. (1996). Towards ecological self: deep ecology meets constructionist
self-theory. J. Environ. Psychol. 16, 93–108. doi: 10.1006/jevp.1996.0008
Bragg, R., and Atkins, G. (2016). A Review of Nature-Based Interventions for Mental
Health Care. Natural England Commissioned Reports.
Braun, V., and Clarke, V. (2006). Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qual. Res.
Psychol. 3, 77–101. doi: 10.1191/1478088706qp063oa
Braun, V., and Clarke, V. (2013). Successful Qualitative Research a Practical Guide
for Beginners. London: SAGE.
Brymer, E., Araújo, D., Davids, K., and Pepping, G.-J. (2020). Conceptualizing
the human health outcomes of acting in natural environments: an ecological
perspective. Front. Psychol. 11:1362. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01362
Brymer, E., Crabtree, J., and King, R. (2021). Exploring perceptions of how nature
recreation benefits mental wellbeing: a qualitative enquiry. Ann. Leisure Res.
doi: 10.1080/11745398.2020.1778494
Carey, T. A. (2006). The Method of Levels: How to do Psychotherapy Without Getting
in the Way. Hayward, CA: Living Control Systems Publishing.
Carey, T. A., Mansell, W., and Tai, S. J. (2014). A biopsychosocial model
based on negative feedback and control. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:94.
doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00094
Case, C. (2005). Imagining Animals: Art, Psychotherapy and Primitive States of
Mind. London; New York, NY: Taylor & Francis.
Chawla, L. (2020). Childhood nature connection and constructive hope: a review of
research on connecting with nature and coping with environmental loss. People
Nat. 2, 619–642. doi: 10.1002/pan3.10128
Coventry, P. A., Neale, C., Dyke, A., Pateman, R., and Cinderby, S. (2019). The
mental health benefits of purposeful activities in public green spaces in urban
and semi-urban neighbourhoods: a mixed-methods pilot and proof of concept
study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 16:2712. doi: 10.3390/ijerph1615
2712
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org
12
March 2021 | Volume 12 | Article 580992
Iqbal and Mansell
Nature Engagement Pathways
de Bloom, J., Sianoja, M., Korpela, K., Tuomisto, M., Lilja, A., Geurts, S., et al.
(2017). Effects of park walks and relaxation exercises during lunch breaks on
recovery from job stress: two randomized controlled trials. J. Environ. Psychol.
51, 14–30. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2017.03.006
Deci, E. L., and Ryan, R. M. (1985). The general causality orientations
scale: self-determination in personality. J. Res. Pers. 19, 109–134.
doi: 10.1016/0092-6566(85)90023-6
Diener, E., Suh, E., Lucas, R., and Smith, H. (1999). Subjective
wellbeing: three decades of progress. Psychol. Bull. 125, 276–302.
doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.125.2.276
Ewert, A. W., Mitten, D. S., and Overholt, J. R. (2014). Natural Environments and
Human Health. CABI. doi: 10.1079/9781845939199.0000
Ferraro, F. M. III. (2015). Enhancement of convergent creativity
following a multiday wilderness experience. Ecopsychology 7, 7–11.
doi: 10.1089/eco.2014.0043
Ferrer, J. N. (2002). Revisioning Transpersonal Theory: A Participatory Vision of
Human Spirituality. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.
Genter, C., Roberts, A., Richardson, J., and Sheaff, M. (2015). The contribution
of allotment gardening to health and wellbeing: a systematic review of the
literature. Br. J. Occup. Ther. 78, 593–605. doi: 10.1177/0308022615599408
Gibson, J. J. (1979). The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception. Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Glasser, W. (1981). Stations of the Mind: New Directions for Reality Therapy. New
York, NY: Harper Collins.
Greenleaf, A. T., Bryant, R. M., and Pollock, J. B. (2014). Nature-Based counseling:
integrating the healing benefits of nature into practice. Int. J. Adv. Counsel. 36,
162–174. doi: 10.1007/s10447-013-9198-4
Heerwagen, J. H., and Orians, G. H. (2002). “The ecological world of children,” in
Children and Nature: Psychological, Sociocultural and Evolutionary Perspectives,
eds P. H. Kahn. Jr, and S. R. Kellert (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 29–64.
Howell, A. J., Dopko, R. L., Passmore, H. A., and Buro, K. (2011). Nature
connectedness: associations with wellbeing and mindfulness. Pers. Individ. Dif.
51, 166–171. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.03.037
Immonen, T., Brymer, E., Davids, K., Liukkonen, J., and Jaakkola, T. (2018).
An ecological conceptualization of extreme sports. Front. Psychol. 9, 1274.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01274
Jordan, J. S. (2008). Wild agency: nested intentionalities in cognitive neuroscience
and archaeology. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 363, 1981–1991.
doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0009
Jordan, M. (2014). Nature and Therapy: Understanding Counselling
and Psychotherapy in Outdoor Spaces. London: Routledge.
doi: 10.4324/9781315752457
Kamitsis, I., and Francis, A. J. (2013). Spirituality mediates the relationship
between engagement with nature and psychological wellbeing. J. Environ.
Psychol. 36, 136–143. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2013.07.013
Kaplan, R., and Kaplan, S. (1989). The Experience of Nature: A Psychological
Perspective. New York, NY: CUP Archive.
Kaplan, S. (1995). The restorative benefits of nature: toward
an integrative framework. J. Environ. Psychol. 15, 169–182.
doi: 10.1016/0272-4944(95)90001-2
Kellert, S. R., and Wilson, E. O. (1995). The Biophilia Hypothesis. Covelo, CA:
Island Press.
Kok, B. E., and Fredrickson, B. L. (2010). Upward spirals of the heart: autonomic
flexibility, as indexed by vagal tone, reciprocally and prospectively predicts
positive emotions and social connectedness. Biol. Psychol. 85, 432–436.
doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.09.005
Korpela, K. (1995). Developing the Environmental Self-Regulation Hypothesis.
Tampere: Acta Universitatis Tamperensis.
Kuo, M., Browning, M. H., and Penner, M. L. (2018). Do lessons in nature boost
subsequent classroom engagement? Refueling students in flight. Front. Psychol.
8:2253. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02253
Landon, A. C., Woosnam, K. M., Kyle, G. T., and Keith, S. J. (2020). Psychological
needs satisfaction and attachment to natural landscapes. Environ. Behav.
doi: 10.1177/0013916520916255
Larson, J. S. (1993). The measurement of social wellbeing. Soc. Indic. Res. 28,
285–296. doi: 10.1007/BF01079022
Leong, L. Y. C., Fischer, R., and McClure, J. (2014). Are nature lovers
more innovative? The relationship between connectedness with nature and
cognitive styles. J. Environ. Psychol. 40, 57–63. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2014.
03.007
Lupton, D., and Tulloch, J. (2002). ’Life would be pretty dull without
risk’: voluntary risk-taking and its pleasures. Health Risk Soc. 4, 113–124.
doi: 10.1080/13698570220137015
MacIntyre, T. E., Beckmann, J., Calogiuri, G., Donnell, A. A., Jones, M.
V., Madan, C. R., et al. (2020). Editorial: human-nature interactions:
perspectives on conceptual and methodological issues. Front. Psychol.
11:607888. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.607888
Mansell, W. (2018). Method of levels: is it the most parsimonious psychological
therapy available? Rev. Psicoter. 29, 135–143. doi: 10.33898/rdp.v29i110.242
Mansell, W. (2020). “Goals and emotions,” in The Routledge International
Encyclopedia of Sport and Exercise Psychology, Vol. 1., eds D. Hackfort and R. J.
Schinke (London, New York, NY: Routledge). doi: 10.4324/9781315187259-13
Mansell, W., and Hodson, S. (2009). “Imagery and memories of the social self in
people with bipolar disorders: empirical evidence, phenomenology, theory and
therapy,” in Imagery and the Threatened Self, ed L. Stopa (Hove: Routledge).
Mansell, W., and Huddy, V. (2020). Why do we need computational models of
psychological change and recovery, and how should they be designed and
tested? Front. Psychiatry 11:624. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00624
Martyn, P., and Brymer, E. (2016). The relationship between nature relatedness
and anxiety. J. Health Psychol. 21, 1436–1445. doi: 10.1177/1359105314555169
Mayer, F. S., and Frantz, C. M. (2004). The connectedness to nature scale: a
measure of individuals’ feeling in community with nature. J. Environ. Psychol.
24:503–515. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2004.10.001
McClelland, K. (2020). “:Social structure and control: perceptual control theory
and the science of sociology,” in The Interdisciplinary Handbook of Perceptual
Control Theory, ed W. Mansell (London; San Diego, CA; Cambridge, MA;
Oxford: Academic Press), 229–297. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-818948-1.00009-5
McCree, M., Cutting, R., and Sherwin, D. (2018). The hare and the tortoise
go to forest school: taking the scenic route to academic attainment
via emotional wellbeing outdoors. Early Child Dev. Care 188, 980–996.
doi: 10.1080/03004430.2018.1446430
McMahan, E. A., and Estes, D. (2015). The effect of contact with natural
environments on positive and negative affect: a meta-analysis. J. Posit. Psychol.
10, 507–519. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2014.994224
Nguyen, J., and Brymer, E. (2018). Nature-based guided imagery as an intervention
for state anxiety. Front. Psychol. 9:1858. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01858
O’Brien, L., Townsend, M., and Ebden, M. (2010). ‘Doing something positive’:
Volunteers’ experiences of the wellbeing benefits derived from practical
conservation activities in nature. Voluntas Int. J. Volunt. Nonprofit Organ. 21,
525–545. doi: 10.1007/s11266-010-9149-1
Pasanen, T., Johnson, K., Lee, K., and Korpela, K. (2018). Can nature walks with
psychological tasks improve mood, self-reported restoration, and sustained
attention? Results from two experimental field studies. Front. Psychol. 9:2057.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02057
Pasanen, T. P., Neuvonen, M., and Korpela, K. M. (2018). The psychology of
recent nature visits: (how) are motives and attentional focus related to post-visit
restorative experiences, creativity, and emotional wellbeing? Environ. Behav.
50, 913–944. doi: 10.1177/0013916517720261
Passmore, H. A., and Holder, M. D. (2017). Noticing nature: individual and
social benefits of a two-week intervention. J. Posit. Psychol. 12, 537–546.
doi: 10.1080/17439760.2016.1221126
Pellis, S. M., and Bell, H. C. (2020). “Unraveling the dynamics of dyadic
interactions: perceptual control in animal contests,” in The Interdisciplinary
Handbook of Perceptual Control Theory, ed W. Mansell (London;
San Diego, CA; Cambridge, MA; Oxford: Academic Press), 75–99.
doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-818948-1.00005-8
Plooij, F. X. (2020). “The phylogeny, ontogeny, causation and function of
regression periods explained by reorganizations of the hierarchy of perceptual
control systems,” in The Interdisciplinary Handbook of Perceptual Control
Theory, ed W. Mansell (London; San Diego, CA; Cambridge, MA; Oxford:
Academic Press), 199–225. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-818948-1.00008-3
Powers, W. T. (1973). Behavior: The Control of Perception. New York,
NY: Hawthorne.
Powers, W. T. (1998). Making Sense of Behavior. New Canaan, CT: Benchmark.
Powers, W. T. (2008). Living Control Systems III: The fact of control. New Caan,
CT: Benchmark Publications Inc.
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org
13
March 2021 | Volume 12 | Article 580992
Iqbal and Mansell
Nature Engagement Pathways
Powers, W. T., Clark, R. K., and McFarland, R. L. (1960a). A general
feedback theory of human behavior. Part I Percept. Motor Skills 11, 71–88.
doi: 10.2466/pms.1960.11.1.71
Powers, W. T., Clark, R. K., and McFarland, R. L. (1960b). A general feedback
theory of human behavior. Part II Percept. Motor Skills 11, 309–323.
doi: 10.2466/pms.1960.11.3.309
Purcell, T., Peron, E., and Berto, R. (2001). Why do preferences differ between
scene types? Environ. Behav. 33, 93–106. doi: 10.1177/00139160121972882
Ratcliffe, E., and Korpela, K. M. (2018). Time-and self-related memories predict
restorative perceptions of favorite places via place identity. Environ. Behav. 50,
690–720. doi: 10.1177/0013916517712002
Richardson, M. (2019). Beyond restoration: considering emotion regulation in
natural wellbeing. Ecopsychology 11, 123–129. doi: 10.1089/eco.2019.0012
Richardson, M., and McEwan, K. (2018). 30 days wild and the relationships
between engagement with nature’s beauty, nature connectedness and well-
being. Front. Psychol. 9:1500. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01500
Richardson, M., and Sheffield, D. (2015). Reflective self-attention: a more stable
predictor of connection to nature than mindful attention. Ecopsychology 7,
166–175. doi: 10.1089/eco.2015.0010
Ryff, C., and Keyes, C. (1995). The structure of psychological wellbeing revisited. J.
Pers. Soc. Psychol. 69, 719–727. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.69.4.719
Schultz, P. W. (2002). “Inclusion with nature: the psychology of human-
nature relations,” in Psychology of Sustainable Development, eds P.
Schmuck, and W. P. Schultz (Norwell, MA: Kluwer Academic), 62–78.
doi: 10.1007/978-1-4615-0995-0_4
Schutte, N. S., and Malouff, J. M. (2018). Mindfulness and connectedness
to nature: a meta-analytic investigation. Pers. Individ. Dif. 127, 10–14.
doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2018.01.034
Schweitzer, R., Glab, H., and Brymer, E. (2018). The human-nature
relationship: a phenomenological-relational perspective. Front. Psychol.
9:969. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00969
Shanahan, D. F., Astell–Burt, T., Barber, E. A., Brymer, E., Cox, D. T., Dean, J., et al.
(2019). Nature–based interventions for improving health and wellbeing: the
purpose, the people and the outcomes. Sports 7:141. doi: 10.3390/sports7060141
Stevenson, M. P., Schilhab, T., and Bentsen, P. (2018). Attention restoration
theory II: a systematic review to clarify attention processes affected by exposure
to natural environments. J. Toxicol. Environ. Health Part B 21, 227–268.
doi: 10.1080/10937404.2018.1505571
Trigwell, J. L., Francis, A. J., and Bagot, K. L. (2014). Nature connectedness and
eudaimonic wellbeing: spirituality as a potential mediator. Ecopsychology 6,
241–251. doi: 10.1089/eco.2014.0025
Ulrich, R. S. (1979). Visual landscapes and psychological well-
being. Landsc. Res. 4, 17–23. doi: 10.1080/014263979087
05892
Ulrich, R. S. (1983). “Aesthetic and affective response to natural environment,”
in Behavior and the Natural Environment (Boston, MA: Springer), 85–125.
doi: 10.1007/978-1-4613-3539-9_4
Ulrich, R. S., Simons, R. F., Losito, B. D., Fiorito, E., Miles, M. A., and Zelson, M.
(1991). Stress recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments. J.
Environ. Psychol. 11, 201–230. doi: 10.1016/S0272-4944(05)80184-7
Wilson, E. O. (1984). Biophilia: The Human Bond With Other Species. Cambridge:
Harvard University Press.
Windhorst, E., and Williams, A. (2015). “It’s like a different world”: natural
places, post-secondary students, and mental health. Health Place 34, 241–250.
doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2015.06.002
Wolsko, C., and Lindberg, K. (2013). Experiencing connection with nature:
the matrix of psychological wellbeing, mindfulness, and outdoor recreation.
Ecopsychology 5, 80–91. doi: 10.1089/eco.2013.0008
Wooller, J. J., Rogerson, M., Barton, J., Micklewright, D., and Gladwell, V. (2018).
Can simulated green exercise improve recovery from acute mental stress? Front.
Psychol. 9:2167. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02167
Yin, H. H. (2017). The basal ganglia in action. Neuroscientist 23, 299–313.
doi: 10.1177/1073858416654115
Yin, J., Yuan, J., Arfaei, N., Catalano, P. J., Allen, J. G., and Spengler, J. D.
(2020). Effects of biophilic indoor environment on stress and anxiety recovery:
a between-subjects experiment in virtual reality. Environ. Int. 136:105427.
doi: 10.1016/j.envint.2019.105427
Zhang, J. W., Howell, R. T., and Iyer, R. (2014). Engagement with natural
beauty moderates the positive relation between connectedness with
nature and psychological wellbeing. J. Environ. Psychol. 38, 55–63.
doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2013.12.013
Conflict of Interest: The authors declare that the research was conducted in the
absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a
potential conflict of interest.
Copyright © 2021 Iqbal and Mansell. This is an open-access article distributed
under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use,
distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original
author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication
in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use,
distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org
14
March 2021 | Volume 12 | Article 580992