The Relationship between Landscape Metrics and Facial Expressions in 18 Urban Forest Parks of Northern China
Article
The Relationship between Landscape Metrics and Facial
Expressions in 18 Urban Forest Parks of Northern China
Ping Liu 1,†, Mengnan Liu 1,† , Tingting Xia 1, Yutao Wang 1,* and Peng Guo 2,*
1 College of Forestry, Shenyang Agricultural University, Shenyang 110866, China; lp_79@syau.edu.cn (P.L.);
lmndeyouxiang@163.com (M.L.); xtt3939@163.com (T.X.)
2 Environment and Resources College, Dalian Nationalities University, Dalian 116600, China
* Correspondence: ytw730@syau.edu.cn (Y.W.); gp@dlnu.edu.cn (P.G.);
Tel.: +86-024-8848-7150 (Y.W.); +86-158-4086-3291 (P.G.)
† These authors contributed equally to this work.
Citation: Liu, P.; Liu, M.; Xia, T.;
Wang, Y.; Guo, P. The Relationship
between Landscape Metrics and
Facial Expressions in 18 Urban Forest
Parks of Northern China. Forests 2021,
12, 1619. https://doi.org/10.3390/
f12121619
Abstract: Urban forests are an important green infrastructure that positively impacts human well-
being by improving emotions and reducing psychological stress. Questionnaires have been used
frequently to study the influence of forest experiences on mental health; however, they have poor
controllability and low accuracy for detecting immediate emotions. This study used the alternative
approach of facial reading, detecting the facial expressions of urban forest visitors and their rela-
tionships with the landscape metrics. Using the microblogging site, Sina Weibo, we collected facial
photos of 2031 people visiting 18 different forest parks across Northern China in 2020. We used
satellite imagery analysis to assess the elevation and pattern sizes of green space and blue space
areas. Age and location were taken as independent variables affecting facial expressions, which were
categorized as happy or sad. With increases in green space and intact park areas, people showed a
higher frequency of expressing happy scores. The results showed that the forest experience frequently
elicited positive emotions, suggesting that creating and maintaining urban green spaces enhance
people’s quality of life.
Keywords: urban forest landscape; forest therapy; facial expressions
Academic Editors: Hongxu Wei and
Richard Hauer
Received: 8 November 2021
Accepted: 22 November 2021
Published: 24 November 2021
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral
with regard to jurisdictional claims in
published maps and institutional affil-
iations.
Copyright: © 2021 by the authors.
Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland.
This article is an open access article
distributed under the terms and
conditions of the Creative Commons
Attribution (CC BY) license (https://
creativecommons.org/licenses/by/
4.0/).
1. Introduction
More than half the world’s population lives in urban areas, which studies have shown
can negatively affect mental health [1]. Studies have also shown that contact with beauty
and nature has physical and mental health benefits, including stress reduction [2–4]. Forest
Park sustainability and visitors’ park experiences and well-being depend on park managers’
correlative decisions and the parks’ locations [5]. People living in forest-rich areas have
increased opportunities for stress-reducing experiences in natural settings [6]. Research
has also shown that while age can affect park visitors’ emotions and forest experiencers [7],
comfortable forest environments induce positive emotions and reduce anxiety in young
adults [8], middle-aged and elderly visitors, and visitors with dementia [9]. Furthermore,
short-term exposure to an urban forest environment has been shown to significantly lower
the heart rates of middle-aged men with hypertension and older adults [10,11]. Most previ-
ous research has used quantitative physiological measures or qualitative descriptions (e.g.,
questionnaires, interviews) to analyse urban parks’ effects on mental health. This study
tested the objectivity of previous studies’ results by using an alternative measure, facial
expressions, detecting the emotional responses of different types of visitors to different
urban forest environments.
Optimal combinations of natural landscape elements (e.g., water, plants, topography),
characteristics (e.g., location, appearance, art, and cultural elements), and park dimensions
provide restorative effects that induce resilience [12] and benefit physical and mental
health [13–15]. Studies have found that urban residents with ready access to higher
Forests 2021, 12, 1619. https://doi.org/10.3390/f12121619
https://www.mdpi.com/journal/forests
Forests 2021, 12, 1619
2 of 16
densities of green space and vegetation were less likely to suffer from heart disease [16]
and mental illness [17]. The number, size, accessibility, vegetation, and features of public
green spaces can dramatically affect residents’ mental health [18]. Especially conducive
to relaxation are blue spaces—surface waterbodies (e.g., lakes, ponds, wetlands), surface
watercourses (e.g., rivers, streams, estuaries), and other aquatic landscapes (e.g., beaches,
coasts) [19]. A Finnish study found that people’s favourite restorative experiences involved
exercise and activity in outdoor areas, waterside environments, and extensively managed
natural settings [20]. Experiencing blue spaces can improve mental health and promote
physical activity [21]. Natural environments with aquatic elements evoke more positive
emotions than those without blue spaces [22]. To figure the mechanism for impacts of
green space and blue space on public mental health is a requisite to compare their positive
effects on mental well-being and are beneficial for better choices, such as the experience to
touch the nature.
This study focused on urban parks, which provide opportunities for urban residents
and visitors to engage in physical activity in managed natural settings in cities and enjoy
the restorative effects of nature [23]. Parks located close to residences generally have
more visitors [24]. Urban parks’ accessibility, area, size, and landscape style can influence
visitors’ impressions, activities, and mental well-being [25,26]. Brown et al. [27] found that
more frequent park use was associated with park size and suitability for physical activity.
Even small parks and pocket parks can positively impact visitors’ mental health [28].
Parks’ terrain and altitude can affect visitors’ experiences. Shukitt and Bandaret reported
that people’s behaviours and moods differ at different altitudes; they can become more
argumentative and irritable or more euphoric at high altitudes [29]. Dudek found that the
terrain’s slope was closely related to the aesthetics of forest landscapes; for example, tree
species with high aesthetic landscape values mainly grow on high, concentrated slopes
of 8–12[30]. Thus, park visitors’ emotional states can vary depending on the parks’
accessibility, area, size, scale, landscape style, design features, elevation, and topography.
Geographic information systems (GIS) provide spatial decision support for forestry
and park management and planning [31]. GIS data can provide valuable quantitative and
timely information for mapping and assessing changes in landscapes and green spaces [32].
Stored satellite imagery can provide decades of consistent, precise, high-quality images [33]
and detailed landscape pattern records at different temporal and spatial scales [34]. Data
from the joint National Aeronautics and Space Administration (NASA) and United States
Geological Survey (USGS) Landsat program have become increasingly critical of extensive
studies on landscape dynamics [35]. The quantitative approach at a global scale relies on
GIS, high-resolution digital elevation models (DEMs), and digital aerial imagery [36], which
require neither public preferences nor large-scale research or measures [37] to identify and
monitor precise locations over time.
Emotions can be observed through external physical expressions and monitored by
measuring physiological changes and internal sensations [38]. Palermo and Rhodes wrote
that “faces are probably the most biologically and socially significant visual stimuli in the
human environment” [39] (p. 75), requiring us to rapidly detect, categorize, and interpret
facial expressions as clues about others’ emotions, behaviours, and intentions. People’s
facial expressions vary widely and can depend on many factors, including their emotions,
social setting, environment, health, cultural background, and personality [40]. Emotional
perception is usually expressed through multiple sensory channels, with crosstalk between
the channels (e.g., sight, smell, touch, taste, hearing) [41]. For example, music can affect
emotions (and thus facial expressions) through auditory, visual, and touch sensations [42].
Responses can be subjective, but pleasant smells induce positive emotions, and unpleasant
smells induce negative emotions [43]. Therefore, perceptions of urban parks can be affected
by numerous sensory perceptions, such as sights, sounds, smells, proprioception (body
position), temperature, and humidity [44]. We posited that assessing park visitors’ facial
expressions could better clarify the influence of urban forest environments on mental health
and well-being.
Forests 2021, 12, 1619
3 of 16
Traditional research has relied primarily on self-reported scores to assess visitors’
emotions about the forest experience [45]. However, surveys require a significant time
commitment from participants, and it can be challenging to recruit enough participants
to validate the survey. We proposed an alternative method to test urban forest park
visitors’ psychological responses: facial reading. This relatively new technique that uses a
software algorithm trained to assess emotion expressions using visual records of human
faces [46]. Our method of facial emotion recognition followed four steps: (1) detecting facial
images; (2) processing the photos to ensure that they were clear and without excessive
modification; (3) extracting facial features; and (4) classifying the facial expressions and
scoring the expressions [47]. Previous studies have demonstrated that facial reading
techniques can be used to assess people’s emotional states in urban forests. For example,
Wei et al. [44] used facial reading technology to investigate the impact of forest experiences
on visitors’ emotional states. Current facial recognition technology has achieved 87%
accuracy in categorizing human facial expressions by emotion [48]. The use of facial
reading technology significantly contributes to the real-time detection of park visitors’
facial expressions, providing a measure of their emotional responses to the park.
Our study analysed the relationship between visitors’ facial expressions and urban
forest landscape metrics in Northern China. We calculated the metrics of green spaces,
blue spaces, park features, and park elevations to test the relationship between three
expressions—happy, sad, and neutral—and the positive response index. We hypothesized
that an increase in the combined green space area, blue space area, overall park area, and
park elevation would arouse higher positive emotions in visitors. This study’s findings
will provide a useful reference for park visitors’ emotional responses to urban forests to
inform the design and construction of future urban forest parks.
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Study Sites
This study focused on nine cities located in Northern China (Table 1). As the centres
of China’s provinces, provincial capitals have a high status and momentous influence on
regional economic development, driving and promoting the development of local and
neighbouring areas [49]. Hence, we selected two forest parks in nine provincial capital
cities in Northern China as the research sites, randomly selecting two forest parks with
different landscapes in each city. Figure 1 shows the specific geographical distribution of
the studied forest locations.
Table 1. Summary of information about forest parks and the number of photos in northern China in 2020.
City
Huhhot
Taiyuan
Shijiazhuang
Beijing
Tianjin
Urumqi
Shenyang
Changchun
Harbin
Forest Park
1. Hadamen National Forest Park
2. Daqingshan Wildlife Park
3. Taiyuan Forest Park
4. Wenying Park
5. Century Park
6. Xiushui Park
7. Olympic Forest Park
8. Grand Canal Forest Park
9. Pak Ning Park
10. Tanggu Forest Park
11. People’s park
12. Tianshan Canyon
13. Beiling Park
14. Changbai Island Forest Park
15. Jingyuetan Scentic Spot
16. South Lake Park
17. Heilongjiang Forest Botanical Garden
18. Sun Island Park
Coordinate
4101 N, 11158 E
4088 N, 11162 E
3791 N, 11254 E
3787 N, 11257 E
3802 N, 11454 E
3809 N, 11439 E
4002 N, 11639 E
3988 N, 11674 E
3917 N, 11722 E
3910 N, 11767 E
4380 N, 8761 E
4349 N, 8744 E
4185 N, 12343 E
4175 N, 12339 E
4378 N, 12548 E
4386 N, 12531 E
4571 N, 12665 E
4579 N, 12660 E
Number of Photos
53
100
178
75
111
86
76
142
90
125
91
134
133
141
180
160
43
113
Forests 2021, 12, 1619
4 of 16
Figure 1. Distributions of study areas (A) in northern China. Forest park locations are labelled by green dots and red dots in
north China (B), northwest China (C), and northeast China (D).
2.2. Data Source
2.2.1. Photo Download and Treatment
We used the microblogging site Sina Weibo (Sina Corporation, Beijing, China) as the
photo data source since the information was completely accessible to the public and the
photos contained geolocation data [50]. Weibo is the largest social network service (SNS)
platform in China, publishing the largest number of microblogs by Chinese users [51].
The study focused on visitors with typical oriental facial features, collecting 2031 photos
to test the facial emotional expression of those visitors from 1 January to 31 December
2020, from 18 different urban forest parks. Our procedures and requirements for collecting
photos for academic use were in full accordance with the ethical standards of the College
of Forestry, Shenyang Agricultural University, China (CF-EC-2021-001). We used the data
only to categorize the facial expressions as happy, sad, or neutral; this data could only be
used for academic research, not any business initiative.
We downloaded and processed the photographs following these steps. First, we
collected all the microblog photographs that showed oriental faces and contained check-in
(geolocation) information relevant to one of the 18 urban forest parks between 1 January to
31 December 2020. Second, we screened the collected photos to select only photos where
people’s facial elements (e.g., eyes, eyebrows, nose, mouth, ears) were clear and not overly
decorated or covered. Third, we cropped the photos so that each photo contained only one
face, with the nose axis perpendicular to the horizontal plane. Finally, we marked all the
processed photos with the geolocation data, the date they were taken, and the person’s age
(i.e., toddler, youth, adult, old).
Forests 2021, 12, 1619
5 of 16
2.2.2. Landscape Metric Collection and Treatment
We used the ArcGIS software to accurately outline the green spaces, blues spaces, and
park boundaries in the Landsat image of each forest park. We then calculated the average
elevation of the park. These processes required first performing projection conversion on
the Landsat data so we could accurately outline of the green spaces, blue spaces, and park
boundaries. We used DEM images to randomly select multiple points on the cropped park
layer to calculate the average elevation of each forest park. Finally, we studied the green
space, blue space, park area, and average park height as landscape metrics.
2.3. Facial Expression Analysis
We analysed the processed photos using the FireFACETM facial recognition software,
Version 1.0 (Zhilunpudao Agricultural Science & Technique Inc., Changchun, China),
assigning each face a happy, sad, or neutral expression score. We also used the positive
response index (PRI) as a score to assess visitors’ net positive emotions [48]. We calculated
this variable as the happy score minus the sad score, the visitors’ immediate net positive
emotions [44]. We calibrated the FireFACETM software by training it with oriental faces
with deliberately posed happy, sad, and neutral facial expressions. We considered the
training complete when the software could correctly identify 80% of the happy or sad faces
and 85% of the neutral faces [52]. These three expressions were the only expressions used
in the study’s analyses because they had the highest accuracy after testing and could be
matched reliably to the landscape metrics for analysis.
2.4. Statistical Analysis
We used IBM® SPSS® Statistics for Windows, Version 26.0 (IBM Corp., Armonk,
NY, USA) for our data analysis. In the analysis of variance (ANOVA) on the expression
scores, the following variables were fixed effects: age (toddler, youth, middle, old) and
city (Huhhot, Taiyuan, Shijiazhuang, Beijing, Tianjin, Ürümqi, Shenyang, Changchun, and
Harbin). When the data did not follow a normal distribution, we ranked the expression
scores according to the order of variance to make them distribution-free. In the ANOVA
analysis of the landscape metric, we tested the differences in the forest parks’ landscape
metrics in the different cities, using the mean value ± standard deviation (SD) to express
the results. When a significant effect was shown, we used the least significant difference
(LSD) test with a significance level of 0.05 for comparison to prevent unevenness in the
number of repetitions between the data groups. The original data used Spearman’s rank
correlation coefficient analysis to evaluate the relationship between the landscape metrics
(independent variables) and the happy, sad, and neutral expression scores and the well as
PRI (dependent variables).
3. Results
3.1. Landscape Metric among Different Northern Cities Analysis
Table 2 shows the landscape metrics of the 18 forest parks in the nine capital cities in
Northern China, Figure 2 shows the mean distribution of the green spaces, blue spaces,
park area, and average park elevation of each urban forest park.
We found significant differences in the green spaces, blue spaces, park area, and
average park elevation among the nine cities (Table 3). The total green space in Ürümqi
(53,073.25 ± 43,820.81 ha) was significantly higher than in the other cities. In terms of
blue spaces, Changchun (307.94 ± 235.87 ha), Beijing (123.50 ± 59.13 ha), and Harbin
(75.66 ± 45.48 ha) differed significantly from the other five cities. Changchun had the
largest total blue space area, and Huhhot had the smallest (none). Ürümqi had the
largest forest park area (61,895.76 ± 51,066.03 ha), and Shijiazhuang had the smallest
area (45.35 ± 18.83 ha). Our calculations of the forest parks’ average elevation revealed
that the average park elevations in Harbin (122.25 ± 11.37 m), Changchun (233.98 ± 22.96
m), Taiyuan (781.83 ± 4.68 m), Huhhot (1378.31 ± 331.90 m), and Ürümqi (1588.03 ± 592.87
Forests 2021, 12, 1619
6 of 16
m) differed from the other four cities, but there was no significant difference between
Tianjin (4.94 ± 0.30 m), Beijing (17.60 ± 13.34 m), and Shenyang (35.00 ± 2.26 m).
Table 2. Summary of information about forest parks landscape metrics in north of China in 2020.
City
Huhhot
Taiyuan
Shijiazhuang
Beijing
Tianjin
Urumqi
Shenyang
Changchun
Harbin
Forest Park
Hadamen National
Forest Park
Daqingshan Wildlife
Park
Taiyuan Forest Park
Wenying Park
Century Park
Xiushui Park
Olympic Forest Park
Grand Canal Forest
Park
Pak Ning Park
Tanggu Forest Park
People’s park
Tianshan Canyon
Beiling Park
Changbai Island
Forest Park
Jingyuetan Scentic
Spot
South Lake Park
Heilongjiang Forest
Botanical Garden
Sun Island Park
Green Area (ha)
2970.00
521.73
95.90
3.37
11.29
38.29
404.22
546.66
7.74
117.73
15.98
89,104.68
233.99
25.95
6033.38
86.59
86.34
2126.38
Water Area (ha)
none
none
25.73
3.96
3.53
10.90
42.86
166.66
9.55
33.39
1.22
none
26.70
4.19
530.00
58.13
2.16
103.63
Forest Park
Area (ha)
3600.00
820.00
224.00
11.90
28.82
66.70
680.00
713.33
57.87
460.00
30.15
103,848.54
356.74
40.25
9638.00
222.34
136.00
3800.76
Forest Park
Elevation (m)
1832.73
1137.47
778.59
788.83
62.26
84.20
35.81
7.86
4.58
5.20
870.20
2075.52
37.33
32.81
255.61
209.66
140.63
115.26
Figure 2. Cont.
Forests 2021, 12, 1619
7 of 16
Figure 2. Mean distribution of the green area (A), water area (B), park area (C), and park average elevation (D) in northwest,
north and northeast cities in 2020.
Table 3. Analysis of variance (ANOVA) of landscape metrics of green area, water area, park area and park average elevation
among different northern cities.
Variable
Sum of Squares
DF 1
Mean Square
Green area
Water area
Forest-park area
Forest-park
elevation
City Inter-group
City Intra-group
Total
City Intergroup
City Intra-group
Total
City Inter-group
City Intra-group
Total
City Inter-group
City Intra-group
Total
545,980,466,666.19
433,477,010,268.79
979,457,476,934.99
24,308,520.10
20,032,288.71
44,340,808.81
734,483,266,261.66
592,347,112,110.87
1,326,830,378,372.53
642,193,245.86
95,747,708.43
737,940,954.30
8.00
2022.00
2030.00
8.00
2022.00
2030.00
8.00
2022.00
2030.00
8.00
2022.00
2030.00
68,247,558,333.27
214,380,321.60
3,038,565.01
9907.17
91,810,408,282.71
292,951,094.02
80,274,155.73
47,352.97
Note: 1 DF, degree of freedom; 2 Sig, significance, the same below.
Sig. 2
0.000
0.000
0.000
0.000
3.2. Visitors’ Facial Expressions Analysis
3.2.1. Different Ages of Visitors on Facial Expressions Analysis
We categorized the visitors as toddlers (0–5 years), youth (15–25 years), adult
(30–60 years), and old (60+ years) based on their appearance and, when it was avail-
able, data were acquired from the Sina Weibo site. Table 4 shows the results of the ANOVA
of all the visitors’ happy, sad, and neutral expressions. The happy expression scores of
the adult and old visitors were significantly higher than for toddlers and youths. There
were significantly more neutral facial expressions among the toddlers and youth than the
adult and old visitors. There were significantly more sad facial expressions among the old
visitors than the toddler, youth, and adult visitors (Figure 3).
Forests 2021, 12, 1619
8 of 16
Table 4. Analysis of variance (ANOVA) of toddler, youth, middle-aged and old visitors on happy, sad, neutral facial
expressions and positive response index in forest parks.
Source of Variance
Sum of Squares
DF 1
Mean Square
Happy
Sad
Neutral
PRI 3
Age Inter-group
Age Intra-group
Total
Age Inter-group
Age Intra-group
Total
Age Inter-group
Age Intra-group
Total
Age Inter-group
Age Intra-group
Total
45,251.894
3,360,420.586
3,405,672.48
2938.082
518,526.228
521,464.31
43,757.541
2,437,688.961
2,481,446.502
51,707.131
5,315,552.956
5,367,260.086
3
2027
2030
3
2027
2030
3
2027
2030
3
2027
2030
15,083.965
1657.83
979.361
255.81
14,585.847
1202.609
17,235.71
2622.374
Note: 1 DF, degree of freedom; 2 Sig, significance; 3 PRI, positive response index, the same below.
Sig. 2
0.000
0.009
0.000
0.000
Figure 3. The neutral, sad, and happy expression scores of toddlers (A), youth (B), middle-aged (C) and old (D) visitors in
northern forest parks.
Forests 2021, 12, 1619
9 of 16
3.2.2. Different Cities of Visitors on Facial Expressions Analysis
We found significant differences in the happy and neutral scores of visitors’ expres-
sions in China (Table 5). There were significantly higher happy expression scores in Tianjin
than in Taiyuan, Harbin, Changchun, Ürümqi, Shenyang, and Shijiazhuang (Figure 4A).
There were significantly higher neutral scores in Shijiazhuang than in Taiyuan, Beijing,
Huhhot, and Tianjin (Figure 4B). There were no differences in the sadness scores between
cities.
Table 5. Analysis of variance (ANOVA) of cities on happy, sad, neutral facial expressions and positive response index in
forest parks.
Source of Variance
Sum of Squares
DF 1
Mean Square
Happy
Sad
Neutral
PRI 3
City Inter-group
City Intra-group
Total
City Inter-group
City Intra-group
Total
City Inter-group
City Intra-group
Total
City Inter-group
City Intra-group
Total
55,801.983
3,349,870.497
3,405,672.48
2669.126
518,795.184
521,464.31
40,370.52
2,441,075.982
2,481,446.502
74,121.697
5,293,138.389
5,367,260.086
8
2022
2030
8
2022
2030
8
2022
2030
8
2022
2030
6975.248
1656.711
333.641
256.575
5046.315
1207.258
9265.212
2617.774
Note: 1 DF, degree of freedom; 2 Sig, significance; 3 PRI, positive response index, the same below.
Sig. 2
0.000
0.239
0.000
0.000
Figure 4. Happy (A), and neutral (B) scores on visitors’ face in northern cities. Error bars stand for standard errors that
stared from the columns (means). Different letters of a, b and c indicate significant differences of ranked scores according to
LSD test at the 0.05 level.
3.2.3. Cities and Ages Interaction Analysis
We found significant differences between the variables city and age and the visitors’
happy, sad, and neutral facial expressions. When the city variable interacted with age, only
happiness and sadness were significantly different; however, there was no difference in the
neutral expression (Table 6).
Forests 2021, 12, 1619
10 of 16
Table 6. Analysis of variance (ANOVA) with the mixed model of city, age, and their interaction on repeated measures of
ranked scores about happy, sad, and neutral facial expression scores and the positive response index.
Source
Model
City
Age
City × Age
Variable
III Sum of Squares
DF 1
Mean Squares
Happy
Sad
Neutral
PRI 3
Happy
Sad
Neutral
PRI 3
Happy
Sad
Neutral
PRI 3
Happy
Sad
Neutral
PRI 3
147,640.354
19,413.101
111,873.975
219,572.676
38,955.391
9424.833
20,564.952
75,966.925
26,088.062
2551.57
21,006.118
36,141.331
55,217.161
13,185.125
36,484.372
100,695.677
31
4762.592
31
626.229
31
3608.838
31
7082.99
8
4869.424
8
1178.104
8
2570.619
8
9495.866
3
8696.021
3
850.523
3
7002.039
3
12,047.11
20
2760.858
20
659.256
20
1824.219
20
5034.784
Note: 1 DF, degree of freedom; 2 Sig, significance; 3 PRI, positive response index, the same below.
Sig. 2
0.000
0.000
0.000
0.000
0.002
0.000
0.027
0.000
0.001
0.017
0.001
0.003
0.028
0.000
0.060
0.007
3.2.4. Positive Response Index Analysis
Adult visitors had higher PRI scores than toddlers and youth visitors (Figure 5A).
The PRI scores of visitors in Tianjin were significantly higher than in Harbin, Changchun,
Shenyang, Shijiazhuang, and Ürümqi (Figure 5B). The visitors’ PRI scores were positively
correlated with park size (p = 0.011), indicating that people’s net positive emotion levels
were significantly related to the size of the park (Table 7).
Figure 5. PRI of different ages visitors (A) and PRI of visitors from different cities in China (B). Error bars stand for standard
errors that stared from the columns (means). Different letters of a and b indicate significant differences of ranked scores
according to an LSD test at the 0.05 level.
3.3. Landscape Metrics and Facial Expressions Correlation Analysis
The visitors’ happy scores were positively correlated with the green spaces (p = 0.019)
and the park area (p = 0.001); we found the highest number of happy faces in the parks
with the greenest space and overall park area (Table 7). However, there was no correlation
between sad expressions and the four-landscape metrics, suggesting that visitors were not
sadder because a park had less green space, blue space, or overall park area or a specific
Forests 2021, 12, 1619
11 of 16
average park elevation. However, visitors’ neutral expressions were negatively correlated
with a green space area (p = 0.017) and overall park area (p = 0.002). This suggested
that increasing vegetation coverage would not necessarily cause the number of neutral
expressions to have happy (or sad) expressions.
Table 7. Coefficients from Spearman correlation between the landscape metric and three facial expressions and the positive
response index of visitors therein in target forest park.
Index
Happy
Sad
Neutral PRI 1 Green Area Water Area Park Area Park Elevation
Happy
1
Sad
0.731 **
1
Neutral
0.886 ** 0.522 **
1
PRI 1
0.936 ** 0.882 ** 0.768 **
1
Green area
0.052 *
0.001 0.053 * 0.039
1
Water area
0.014
0.012
0.02
0.006
0.324 **
1
Park area
0.071 ** 0.011 0.070 ** 0.056 *
0.984 **
0.319 **
1
Park elevation 0.026
0.015
0.028
0.027
0.286 **
0.422 **
0.261 **
1
Note: * and ** mean significant correlation at p < 0.05 and extremely significant correlation at p < 0.01, respectively. 1 PRI, positive response
index.
4. Discussion
4.1. The Age Effect on Facial Expressions
Thanks to the popularity of mobile devices with high-quality cameras, visitors upload
more selfies than in the past [53]. More of our facial photographs originated with youths’
selfies than other visitors’ selfies. Artificial age assessments have also been used in studies
of public sentiment during pandemics and to provide large-scale data [54]. Toddlers and
youths scored relatively lower than adults and old people on happy expressions. The
sadness scores were lower among adults than among the toddler, youth, and old visitors.
These results suggest that the forest environments had a positive psychological effect
on adult and old visitors, which is consistent with previous research [2]. Therefore, we
concluded that the restorative effects of the urban forest experience extended to the elderly.
The urban park activities provided opportunities for older adults to interact socially while
enjoying the natural environment, enabling them to start and maintain friendships, which
is known to benefit mental health [9]. Because we assessed the emotional expressions
of different age groups during their urban forest park experiences using many sample
photographs, our results provide empirical evidence for the improvement of mental health
through urban parks.
4.2. The Discrepancy of Facial Expressions among Cities
Urbanization is the product of a combination of social, economic, and resource factors.
The rapid development of cities should be based on the science of the human settlement
environment to balance the environmental carrying capacity and urban development
intensity [55]. In 1984, the World Health Organization (WHO) proposed establishing
healthy cities to promote the healthy urban development [56]. The Healthy Cities program
links cities’ living conditions to citizens’ health. Health and well-being are interrelated,
and the environment of a healthy city can influence residents’ and visitors’ health and well-
being. Therefore, psychological variables might be more significant for health than external
variables [57]. Our results showed that parks in Tianjin, Beijing, and Harbin were strongly
correlated with happy expressions; they were the highest in Tianjin. Shijiazhuang had the
lowest happy expression scores. Our results suggest that visitors to northern urban forest
parks showed a higher frequency of happy expressions than visitors to southern urban
forest parks, which is consistent with Wei et al. [46]. Although we found no significant
differences in sad expressions among the nine cities, we found the greatest number in
Shijiazhuang was the most prominent.
Forests 2021, 12, 1619
12 of 16
4.3. Relationship between Landscape Metrics and Facial Expressions
Other studies have focused on specific forest landscape metrics [58]. According to
Chen et al. [59], changes in the landscape scale can affect the visual quality of the visitor
photos. People establish connections with the environment through psychological reac-
tions [60]. Psychophysics quantitatively investigates the relationship between stimuli, the
sensations they elicit, and the magnitude of changes in perceivable physical stimuli [12].
This study explored the impact of various urban forest park stimuli on visitors’ facial
expressions from the perspective of landscape metrics. We found a significant positive
correlation between green spaces and happy expressions. Similarly, Gozalo et al. [61] found
that large green spaces were positively correlated with frequent strolling and relaxation
among urban residents. Therefore, people’s urban forest park experiences might be influ-
enced by lower normalized difference vegetation index (NDVI) scores; NVDI is a graphical
indicator of the live green vegetation and tree cover density. Residents of communities
with abundant green spaces tend to enjoy better mental health. One study in the Nether-
lands found that the positive correlation was most pronounced among senior citizens,
homemakers, and those from lower socioeconomic groups [62]. More research is needed to
verify the findings that green spaces induce positive emotions in visitors. Studies show
that experiencing blue spaces helps reduce stress and promote social contact among the
elderly [63]. Other studies support the health benefits of blue space environments [19,64].
In fact, Sonntag-Ostrom et al. [65] reported that blue spaces were the most restorative
landscapes. Similarly, Nutsford et al. [66] indicated that people’s psychological stress
decreased when they were within 15 km of visible outdoor blue spaces. In our study, there
were three forest environments without water element landscape, which may lead to less
happiness of visitors.
The area and average elevation of forest parks are also crucial factors affecting park
visitors’ expressions. People usually prefer to visit large parks with specific facilities [67].
Another study found that a three-day trek in the highlands could improve children’s
negative moods [68]. We found that the park scale was positively correlated with the PRI
scores. Other studies have shown that humidity, terrain slope, and the vegetation are
also important factors that affect visitors’ experiences [30]. Hence, when planning and
constructing an urban forest park, designers should ensure that the size of the park meets
the residents’ needs to stay active and exercise regularly. They should also consider the
aesthetic value of the park to optimize the benefits of natural surroundings.
4.4. Limitation
We studied the facial expressions of visitors in urban forest parks in nine of China’s
provincial capitals. Future studies should expand the scope of the target city selections and
park locations to eliminate the influence of neighbourhood and inner group differences.
Second, we addressed the effect of the park environments on visitors’ facial expres-
sions, but we did not set up any other urban environment as a control group for comparison.
Future studies should consider additional urban variables to allow comparisons.
Third, people generally make happy expressions when they take selfies or have their
photos taken by others, which might have affected the accuracy of our assessments; visitors
might have worn sad or neutral faces until they were being photographed, which could
have led to inaccuracies in our analyses. Therefore, future research should try to choose
randomly captured candid photos for testing rather than selfies because the expressions
would be more representative than posed photos.
Fourth, our age categorizations were guesses based primarily on people’s appearances.
Liu et al.’s study [51] used visual assessments with the same age ranges: toddlers 0–5,
youths 15–25, adults 30–60, and old people 60+; people who seemed to fall into the gaps
were added to the nearest age group (e.g., someone seven years old would be classified
with the toddlers). We might have misclassified the facial expressions because of the
person’s age or the fluidity of expression; for example, a snapshot of what we classified as
a toddler’s neutral expression might have been a transitional expression between happy
Forests 2021, 12, 1619
13 of 16
and sad. However, there was no accurate indicator for distinguishing the visitors’ ages
without additional information.
Finally, we selected only green spaces, blue spaces, park area, and average park
elevation as the landscape metrics; we did not include hardscapes or structures. Future
studies might follow the lead of Su [69], who divided forest landscapes into coniferous
forest, broad-leaved forest, mixed forest, and static water landscape spaces. Su’s study
found that people’s expressions of excitement and relaxation were more frequent in the
mixed forests than in the other three groups. Therefore, our future studies will consider
the influence of the different landscape elements on visitors’ facial expressions to confirm
the connection between nature and benefits to mental health.
5. Conclusions
We screened and downloaded 2031 facial photographs from Sina Weibo of urban park
visitors in nine provincial capitals in Northern China. We performed facial recognition
on the processed photos using the FireFACETM ver 1.0 software to categorize the faces’
expressions as happy, sad, or neutral. The results showed that the urban parks with the
greatest number of visitors with happy expressions were the parks with the greenest space
and overall area. Thus, we recommend that future urban parks should feature a high
percentage of vegetation to maximize residents’ experiences with managed natural spaces
to improve their health and well-being.
Author Contributions: Conceptualization, P.L. and M.L.; methodology, P.L. and M.L.; software,
M.L.; validation, P.L., M.L. and T.X.; formal analysis, P.L., M.L. and Y.W.; investigation, M.L. and
T.X.; resources, P.L.; data curation, M.L. and T.X.; writing—original draft preparation, P.L. and
M.L.; writing—review and editing, P.L. and M.L.; visualization, P.G.; supervision, Y.W.; project
administration, P.L.; funding acquisition, P.L. All authors have read and agreed to the published
version of the manuscript.
Funding: This research was funded by the Liaoning Key Research and Development Program
(grant number: 2020JH2/10200033, 2021JH2/10200007), the National Natural Science Foundation of
China (grant number: 31771695), and the Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities
(Program for ecology research group) (grant number: 0901-110109).
Institutional Review Board Statement: The study was conducted according to the guidelines of
the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by The Ethic Committee of Human Studies, College of
Forestry, Shenyang Agricultural University (protocol code: CF-EC-2021-001; date: 2 November 2021).
Informed Consent Statement: The study was approved by The Ethnic Committee of Human Studies,
College of Forestry, Shenyang Agricultural University (protocol code: CF-EC-2021-001). All photos
data for this study were obtained through Sina Weibo open source. Humans were informed when
they uploaded photos on this open source platform. The Ethical Approval for Human Studies was
submitted to the Forests submission system.
Data Availability Statement: Not applicable.
Acknowledgments: Authors acknowledge Lingquan Meng for photos treatment.
Conflicts of Interest: The authors declare no conflict of interest.
References
1. McKenzie, K.; Murray, A.; Booth, T. Do urban environments increase the risk of anxiety, depression and psychosis? An
epidemiological study. J. Affect. Disord. 2013, 150, 1019–1024. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
2. Lee, J.; Park, B.-J.; Tsunetsugu, Y.; Ohira, T.; Kagawa, T.; Miyazaki, Y. Effect of forest bathing on physiological and psychological
responses in young Japanese male subjects. Public Health 2011, 125, 93–100. [CrossRef]
3. Shin, W.S.; Yeoun, P.S.; Yoo, R.W.; Shin, C.S. Forest experience and psychological health benefits: The state of the art and future
prospect in Korea. Environ. Health Prev. Med. 2010, 15, 38–47. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
4. Poulsen, D.V.; Stigsdotter, U.K.; Djernis, D.; Sidenius, U. ‘Everything just seems much more right in nature’: How veterans
with post-traumatic stress disorder experience nature-based activities in a forest therapy garden. Health Psychol. Open 2016, 3, 3.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]
Forests 2021, 12, 1619
14 of 16
5. Carson, R.T.; DeShazo, J.; Schwabe, K.A.; Vincent, J.R.; Ahmad, I. Incorporating local visitor valuation information into the design
of new recreation sites in tropical forests. Ecol. Econ. 2015, 120, 338–349. [CrossRef]
6. Bahrini, F.; Bell, S.; Mokhtarzadeh, S. The relationship between the distribution and use patterns of parks and their spatial
accessibility at the city level: A case study from Tehran, Iran. Urban For. Urban Green. 2017, 27, 332–342. [CrossRef]
7. Hong, J.; Park, S.; An, M. Are Forest healing programs useful in promoting children’s emotional welfare? The Interpersonal
relationships of children in foster care. Urban For. Urban Green. 2021, 59, 127034. [CrossRef]
8. Bielinis, E.; Bielinis, L.; Krupin´ ska-Szeluga, S.; Łukowski, A.; Takayama, N. The Effects of a Short Forest Recreation Program on
Physiological and Psychological Relaxation in Young Polish Adults. Forests 2019, 10, 34. [CrossRef]
9. Yu, C.-P.; Lin, C.-M.; Tsai, M.-J.; Tsai, Y.-C.; Chen, C.-Y. Effects of Short Forest Bathing Program on Autonomic Nervous System
Activity and Mood States in Middle-Aged and Elderly Individuals. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2017, 14, 897. [CrossRef]
[PubMed]
10. Song, C.; Ikei, H.; Kobayashi, M.; Miura, T.; Li, Q.; Kagawa, T.; Kumeda, S.; Imai, M.; Miyazaki, Y. Effects of viewing forest
landscape on middle-aged hypertensive men. Urban For. Urban Green. 2017, 21, 247–252. [CrossRef]
11. Kabisch, N.; Püffel, C.; Masztalerz, O.; Hemmerling, J.; Kraemer, R. Physiological and psychological effects of visits to different
urban green and street environments in older people: A field experiment in a dense inner-city area. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2021, 207,
103998. [CrossRef]
12. Deng, L.; Li, X.; Luo, H.; Fu, E.-K.; Ma, J.; Sun, L.-X.; Huang, Z.; Cai, S.-Z.; Jia, Y. Empirical study of landscape types, landscape
elements and landscape components of the urban park promoting physiological and psychological restoration. Urban For. Urban
Green. 2020, 48, 126488. [CrossRef]
13. Wang, R.; Zhao, J.; Meitner, M.J.; Hu, Y.; Xu, X. Characteristics of urban green spaces in relation to aesthetic preference and stress
recovery. Urban For. Urban Green. 2019, 41, 6–13. [CrossRef]
14. Li, Z.; Zhao, W.; Nie, M. Scale Characteristics and Optimization of Park Green Space in Megacities Based on the Fractal
Measurement Model: A Case Study of Beijing, Shanghai, Guangzhou, and Shenzhen. Sustainability 2021, 13, 8554. [CrossRef]
15. Grigsby-Toussaint, D.S.; Chi, S.-H.; Fiese, B.H. Where they live, how they play: Neighborhood greenness and outdoor physical
activity among preschoolers. Int. J. Health Geogr. 2011, 10, 66. [CrossRef]
16. Villeneuve, P.J.; Jerrett, M.; Su, J.G.; Burnett, R.T.; Chen, H.; Wheeler, A.; Goldberg, M.S. A cohort study relating urban green
space with mortality in Ontario, Canada. Environ. Res. 2012, 115, 51–58. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
17. Richardson, E.; Pearce, J.; Mitchell, R.; Kingham, S. Role of physical activity in the relationship between urban green space and
health. Public Health 2013, 127, 318–324. [CrossRef]
18. Wood, L.; Hooper, P.; Foster, S.; Bull, F. Public green spaces and positive mental health—investigating the relationship between
access, quantity and types of parks and mental wellbeing. Health Place 2017, 48, 63–71. [CrossRef]
19. Völker, S.; Kistemann, T. The impact of blue space on human health and well-being—Salutogenetic health effects of inland surface
waters: A review. Int. J. Hyg. Environ. Health 2011, 214, 449–460. [CrossRef]
20. Korpela, K.M.; Ylén, M.; Tyrväinen, L.; Silvennoinen, H. Favorite green, waterside and urban environments, restorative
experiences and perceived health in Finland. Health Promot. Int. 2010, 25, 200–209. [CrossRef]
21. Gascon, M.; Zijlema, W.; Vert, C.; White, M.P.; Nieuwenhuijsen, M. Outdoor blue spaces, human health and well-being: A
systematic review of quantitative studies. Int. J. Hyg. Environ. Health 2017, 220, 1207–1221. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
22. White, M.; Smith, A.; Humphryes, K.; Pahl, S.; Snelling, D.; Depledge, M. Blue space: The importance of water for preference,
affect, and restorativeness ratings of natural and built scenes. J. Environ. Psychol. 2010, 30, 482–493. [CrossRef]
23. Bedimo-Rung, A.L.; Mowen, A.J.; Cohen, D.A. The significance of parks to physical activity and public health: A conceptual
model. Am. J. Prev. Med. 2005, 28, 159–168. [CrossRef]
24. Giles-Corti, B.; Broomhall, M.H.; Knuiman, M.; Collins, C.; Douglas, K.; Ng, K.; Lange, A.; Donovan, R.J. Increasing walking:
How important is distance to, attractiveness, and size of public open space? Am. J. Prev. Med. 2005, 28 (Suppl. 2), 169–176.
[CrossRef]
25. Maller, C.; Townsend, M.; Pryor, A.; Brown, P.; St Leger, L. Healthy nature healthy people: ‘contact with nature’ as an upstream
health promotion intervention for populations. Health Promot. Int. 2006, 21, 45–54. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
26. Sang, Å.O.; Knez, I.; Gunnarsson, B.; Hedblom, M. The effects of naturalness, gender, and age on how urban green space is
perceived and used. Urban For. Urban Green. 2016, 18, 268–276. [CrossRef]
27. Brown, G.; Schebella, M.; Weber, D. Using participatory GIS to measure physical activity and urban park benefits. Landsc. Urban
Plan. 2014, 121, 34–44. [CrossRef]
28. Peschardt, K.K.; Stigsdotter, U.K. Associations between park characteristics and perceived restorativeness of small public urban
green spaces. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2013, 112, 26–39. [CrossRef]
29. Shukitt, B.L.; Banderet, L.E. Mood states at 1600 and 4300 meters terrestrial altitude. Aviat. Space Environ. Med. 1988, 59, 530–532.
30. Dudek, T. Influence of selected features of forests on forest landscape aesthetic value—Example of SE Poland. J. Environ. Eng.
Landsc. Manag. 2018, 26, 275–284. [CrossRef]
31. Hytönen, L.A.; Leskinen, P.; Store, R. A Spatial Approach to Participatory Planning in Forestry Decision Making. Scand. J. For.
Res. 2002, 17, 62–71. [CrossRef]
32. Michalowska, K.; Glowienka, E.; Hejmanowska, B. Temporal Satellite Images in The Process of Automatic Efficient Detection of
Changes of the Baltic Sea Coastal Zone. IOP Conf. Ser. Earth Environ. Sci. 2016, 44, 042019. [CrossRef]
Forests 2021, 12, 1619
15 of 16
33. Skole, D.; Tucker, C. Tropical Deforestation and Habitat Fragmentation in the Amazon: Satellite Data from 1978 to 1988. Science
1993, 260, 1905–1910. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
34. Li, Y.; Liu, M.; Liu, X.; Yang, W.; Wang, W. Characterising three decades of evolution of forest spatial pattern in a major coal-energy
province in northern China using annual Landsat time series. Int. J. Appl. Earth Obs. Geoinf. 2021, 95, 102254. [CrossRef]
35. Hermosilla, T.; Wulder, M.; White, J.; Coops, N.C.; Pickell, P.D.; Bolton, D.K. Impact of time on interpretations of forest
fragmentation: Three-decades of fragmentation dynamics over Canada. Remote. Sens. Environ. 2019, 222, 65–77. [CrossRef]
36. Bohlin, J.; Wallerman, J.; Fransson, J.E.S. Forest variable estimation using photogrammetric matching of digital aerial images in
combination with a high-resolution DEM. Scand. J. For. Res. 2012, 27, 692–699. [CrossRef]
37. Cañas, I.; Ayuga, E.; Ayuga, F. A contribution to the assessment of scenic quality of landscapes based on preferences expressed by
the public. Land Use Policy 2009, 26, 1173–1181. [CrossRef]
38. Pal, S.; Mukhopadhyay, S.; Suryadevara, N. Development and Progress in Sensors and Technologies for Human Emotion
Recognition. Sensors 2021, 21, 5554. [CrossRef]
39. Palermo, R.; Rhodes, G. Are you always on my mind? A review of how face perception and attention interact. Neuropsycholgia
2007, 45, 75–92. [CrossRef]
40. Mumenthaler, C.; Sander, D.; Manstead, A. Emotion Recognition in Simulated Social Interactions. IEEE Trans. Affect. Comput.
2018, 11, 1. [CrossRef]
41. Li, D.; Jia, J.; Wang, X. Unpleasant Food Odors Modulate the Processing of Facial Expressions: An Event-Related Potential Study.
Front. Neurosci. 2020, 14, 686. [CrossRef]
42. Stock, J.V.D.; Peretz, I.; Grèzes, J.; de Gelder, B. Instrumental Music Influences Recognition of Emotional Body Language. Brain
Topogr. 2009, 21, 216–220. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
43. Robin, O.; Alaoui-Ismaïli, O.; Dittmar, A.; Vernet-Maury, E. Basic Emotions Evoked by Eugenol Odor Differ According to the
Dental Experience. A Neurovegetative Analysis. Chem. Senses 1999, 24, 327–335. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
44. Wei, H.; Ma, B.; Hauer, R.J.; Liu, C.; Chen, X.; He, X. Relationship between environmental factors and facial expressions of visitors
during the urban forest experience. Urban For. Urban Green. 2020, 53, 126699. [CrossRef]
45. Lee, J.; Tsunetsugu, Y.; Takayama, N.; Park, B.-J.; Li, Q.; Song, C.; Komatsu, M.; Ikei, H.; Tyrväinen, L.; Kagawa, T.; et al. Influence
of Forest Therapy on Cardiovascular Relaxation in Young Adults. Evid. Based Complement. Altern. Med. 2014, 2014, 1–7. [CrossRef]
[PubMed]
46. Wei, H.; Hauer, R.J.; Chen, X.; He, X. Facial Expressions of Visitors in Forests along the Urbanization Gradient: What Can We
Learn from Selfies on Social Networking Services? Forests 2019, 10, 1049. [CrossRef]
47. Karbauskaite˙ , R.; Sakalauskas, L.; Dzemyda, G. Kriging Predictor for Facial Emotion Recognition Using Numerical Proximities of
Human Emotions. Informatica 2020, 31, 249–275. [CrossRef]
48. Kerrihard, A.L.; Khair, M.B.; Blumberg, R.; Feldman, C.H.; Wunderlich, S.M. The effects of acclimation to the United States and
other demographic factors on responses to salt levels in foods: An examination utilizing face reader technology. Appetite 2017,
116, 315–322. [CrossRef]
49. Wang, M.; Xu, J.-H.; Han, S.-R. A Study on the Economic Effects of Provincial Capital Bias and the Adjustment of Urban
Development Strategy in China. In Proceedings of the 2018 International Conference on Management Science and Engineering
(ICMSE), Frankfurt, Germany, 17–20 August 2018; pp. 390–405. [CrossRef]
50. Dai, L.; Xue, T.; Wu, B.; Rong, X.; Xu, B. Spatiotemporal Structure Features of Network Check-in Activities of Urban Residents and
Their Impacting Factors: A Case Study in Six Urban Districts of Beijing. J. Asian Arch. Build. Eng. 2017, 16, 131–138. [CrossRef]
51. Liu, P.; Liu, M.; Xia, T.; Wang, Y.; Wei, H. Can Urban Forest Settings Evoke Positive Emotion? Evidence on Facial Expressions and
Detection of Driving Factors. Sustainability 2021, 13, 8687. [CrossRef]
52. Wei, H.; Hauer, R.J.; Zhai, X. The Relationship between the Facial Expression of People in University Campus and Host-City
Variables. Appl. Sci. 2020, 10, 1474. [CrossRef]
53. Shah, R.; Tewari, R. Demystifying ‘selfie’: A rampant social media activity. Behav. Inf. Technol. 2016, 35, 864–871. [CrossRef]
54. Zhu, J.; Xu, C. Sina microblog sentiment in Beijing city parks as measure of demand for urban green space during the COVID-19.
Urban For. Urban Green. 2021, 58, 126913. [CrossRef]
55. Li, G.Y.; Chen, J.; Yu, F.F. Explore and Analyse of Urbanization Based on the Residential Environment Scientific Perspective. Adv.
Mater. Res. 2011, 250–253, 2734–2739. [CrossRef]
56. Nam, H.-E.; Lee, M.-R.; Kim, H.-S. The Study on the Relationship between Local Residents’ Perception of a Health-Cities and
Personal Happiness. Korean J. Health Serv. Manag. 2014, 8, 175–185. [CrossRef]
57. Lee, M.; Park, S.; Yoon, K. Do Health Promotion Programs Affect Local Residents’ Emotions? Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Heal.
2019, 16, 549. [CrossRef]
58. Gong, L.; Zhang, Z.; Xu, C. Developing a Quality Assessment Index System for Scenic Forest Management: A Case Study from
Xishan Mountain, Suburban Beijing. Forests 2015, 6, 225–243. [CrossRef]
59. Chen, X.F.; Jia, L.M. Research on evaluation of in-forest landscapes in west Beijing mountain area. Sci. Silvae Sin. 2003, 39, 59–66.
60. Bratman, G.N.; Hamilton, J.P.; Daily, G.C. The impacts of nature experience on human cognitive function and mental health. Ann.
N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2012, 1249, 118–136. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
61. Gozalo, G.R.; Morillas, J.M.B.; González, D.M. Perceptions and use of urban green spaces on the basis of size. Urban For. Urban
Green. 2019, 46, 126470. [CrossRef]
Forests 2021, 12, 1619
16 of 16
62. Maas, J.; Verheij, R.A.; Groenewegen, P.P.; de Vries, S.; Spreeuwenberg, P. Green space, urbanity, and health: How strong is the
relation? J. Epidemiol. Community Health 2006, 60, 587–592. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
63. Chen, Y.; Yuan, Y. The neighborhood effect of exposure to blue space on elderly individuals’ mental health: A case study in
Guangzhou, China. Health Place 2020, 63, 102348. [CrossRef]
64. Foley, R.; Kistemann, T. Blue space geographies: Enabling health in place. Health Place 2015, 35, 157–165. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
65. Sonntag-Öström, E.; Stenlund, T.; Nordin, M.; Lundell, Y.; Ahlgren, C.; Fjellman-Wiklund, A.; Järvholm, L.S.; Dolling, A. “Nature’s
effect on my mind”—Patients’ qualitative experiences of a forest-based rehabilitation programme. Urban For. Urban Green. 2015,
14, 607–614. [CrossRef]
66. Nutsford, D.; Pearson, A.L.; Kingham, S.; Reitsma, F. Residential exposure to visible blue space (but not green space) associated
with lower psychological distress in a capital city. Health Place 2016, 39, 70–78. [CrossRef]
67. Bertram, C.; Meyerhoff, J.; Rehdanz, K.; Wüstemann, H. Differences in the recreational value of urban parks between weekdays
and weekends: A discrete choice analysis. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2017, 159, 5–14. [CrossRef]
68. Chao, C.-C.; Chen, L.H.; Lin, Y.-C.; Wang, S.-H.; Wu, S.-H.; Li, W.-C.; Huang, K.-F.; Chiu, T.-F.; Kuo, I.-C. Impact of a 3-Day
High-Altitude Trek on Xue Mountain (3886 m), Taiwan, on the Emotional States of Children: A Prospective Observational Study.
High Alt. Med. Biol. 2019, 20, 28–34. [CrossRef]
69. Su, J.D. The Effect of Different Forest Landscape Spaces on the Physical and Mental Recovery of College Students; Shenyang Agriculture
University: Shenyang, China, 2020. [CrossRef]