The Effect of Recreation in a Snow-Covered Forest Environment on the Psychological Wellbeing of Young Adults: Randomized Controlled Study
Article
The Eect of Recreation in a Snow-Covered Forest
Environment on the Psychological Wellbeing of
Young Adults: Randomized Controlled Study
Ernest Bielinis 1,* , Adrian Łukowski 2,3 , Aneta Omelan 4, Sergii Boiko 5,
Norimasa Takayama 6 and Donald L. Grebner 7
1 Department of Forestry and Forest Ecology, Faculty of Environmental Management and Agriculture,
University of Warmia and Mazury, Pl. Łódzki 2, 10-727 Olsztyn, Poland
2 Faculty of Forestry, Poznan´ University of Life Sciences, Wojska Polskiego 71c, 60-625 Poznan´ , Poland;
adrian.lukowski@up.poznan.pl
3 Institute of Dendrology Polish Academy of Sciences, Parkowa 5, 62-035 Kórnik, Poland
4 Department of Tourism, Recreation and Ecology, Faculty of Environmental Sciences,
University of Warmia and Mazury, ul. Oczapowskiego 5, 10-719 Olsztyn, Poland;
aneta.omelan@uwm.edu.pl
5 Forest Culture Center in Gołuchów, ul. Działyn´ skich 2, 63-322 Gołuchów, Poland; ternianyn@gmail.com
6 Environmental Planning Laboratory, Department of Forest Management, Forestry and Forest Products
Research Institute in Japan, 1 Matsunosato, Tsukuba, Ibaraki 305-8687, Japan; hanri@pri.arc.go.jp
7 Department of Forestry, Mississippi State University, Box 9681, Mississippi State, MS 39762, USA;
dlg26@msstate.edu
* Correspondence: ernest.bielinis@uwm.edu.pl; Tel.: +48-603-809-211
Received: 2 August 2019; Accepted: 19 September 2019; Published: 20 September 2019
Abstract: Forest recreation can be successfully conducted for the purpose of psychological relaxation,
as has been proven in previous scientific studies. During the winter in many countries, when
snow cover occurs frequently, forest recreation (walking, relaxation, photography, etc.) is common.
Nevertheless, whether forest therapy conducted in a forest environment with a snow cover will also
have a positive eect on psychological indicators remains unknown. Furthermore, male subjects
frequently participate in forest therapy experiments, whereas females are rarely involved. Thus,
in this study, the eectuality of forest recreation during winter and with snow cover was tested on
32 young females. For these reasons, the experiment involved 15 min periods of relaxation in a forest
environment or in an urban street environment, in addition to a pre-test under indoor conditions
(randomized controlled study). Four psychological questionnaires Profile of Mood States (POMS),
Positive and Negative Aect Schedule (PANAS), Restorative Outcome Scale (ROS), Subjective
Vitality Scale (SVS)) were administered to participants before and after interventions. Results
showed that participants’ levels of negative mood, as measured by dierent aspects of the POMS
questionnaire (tension/anxiety, anger/hostility, depression/dejection, confusion, and fatigue), decreased
after exposure to the forest environment. In contrast, both tension/anxiety and anger/hostility increased
in the urban street environment. The indicator of negative aect from the PANAS questionnaire also
increased after exposure to the urban street environment, whereas the indicator of positive aect based
on PANAS was higher in the forest environment than in the urban street environment. Restorativeness
and subjective vitality exhibited higher values after exposure to the forest environment in comparison
to those from the control and pre-test. The changes in these indicators demonstrate that forest
recreation in the snow during winter can significantly increase psychological relaxation in females,
as well as show that recreation can be successfully conducted under these winter conditions.
Forests 2019, 10, 827; doi:10.3390/f10100827
www.mdpi.com/journal/forests
Forests 2019, 10, 827
2 of 17
Keywords: deciduous forest; female; forest bathing; forest therapy; Positive and Negative Aect
Schedule; Profile of Mood States; Restorative Outcome Scale; restoration; Shinrin-Yoku; snow covered
forest; Subjective Vitality Scale; winter
1. Introduction
Forest recreation is an activity engaged for pleasure, and it is done outside in a natural forest
environment [1]. One specific form of this activity is forest recreation focusing on health improvement,
mainly called forest therapy (or forest bathing, or Shinrin-yoku). The importance of forest recreation
for health improvement has increased in recent years, as manifested by an increase in the number
of scientific publications regarding this issue. This popularity is not unfounded; many of these
scientific papers have reported various positive influences of forest recreation interventions on humans
with stress symptoms, both psychological and physiological [2–10]. Many positive outcomes have
been reported in both males and females, including lowered negative and heightened positive
mood states [11], lowered pulse rate and blood pressure [12], reduced rates of hormones involved
in stress [13], increased immunological activity, and increased levels of cells responsible for cancer
resistance [14]. This beneficial impact on health has been observed in Scandinavian and Asian countries,
and various strategies, including forest therapy roads [15] and the forest garden Nacadia® [16], have
been introduced to facilitate using the forest and nature for health recreation purposes. Knowledge
concerning the management of forest environments to best achieve the purpose of forest recreation
and knowledge regarding how and when to organize recreational activities are crucial for interested
subjects (e.g., forest owners, foresters, and therapists).
Previous research has confirmed that forest recreation may also be successfully conducted
during the winter and still exhibit beneficial eects on mental health. One previous study observed
psychological relaxation in male and female participants; however, the study was conducted during a
period without snow cover, and any additional eects of snow cover have not been examined [17].
Participants in preference tests positively evaluated winter landscapes of forests with snow, but the
potential of this environment to induce psychological relaxation has not been determined [18]. Thus,
the eectuality of forest recreation on psychological relaxation was tested in the current study. Snow in
a forest may influence the visual properties of the landscape: The ground is covered, plants growing
on the forest floor are not visible, the branches of standing trees are covered, and the dominant color of
scenery is changed from green and brown to white. Furthermore, a layer of snow can aect the process
of visual stimulation. In other studies, dierent indices of greens induced dierent eects on relaxation,
with lower amounts of observed greens resulting in lower levels of relaxation in participants [19]. Thus,
hypothetically, a forest covered by snow might not have the same restorative eect because greens are
hidden. A lack of relaxing eect of the forest environment on respondents may therefore be expected
if the forest is snow-covered. Nevertheless, standing trees continue to fill the forest environment,
regardless of snow, and this could still stimulate relaxation. One of the aims of this study was therefore
to test the influence of a forest with snow cover on psychological relaxation. Eects were tested with
the participation of females (20.97 ± 0.65 years)—a group which has not frequently been represented
in research regarding forest recreation. This study thus provided a good opportunity to examine the
influence of the forest environment on psychological relaxation in this under-represented group.
Overall, the purpose of this work was to test the hypothesis that short periods of forest recreation
could induce the psychological relaxation of participants in a forest environment with snow cover. If this
hypothesis is confirmed, this would suggest that forest recreation could also be successfully conducted
under snowy winter conditions. To examine this hypothesis, an experiment was designed with an
urban street environment as a control and a snow-covered forest environment as an experimental group.
Forests 2019, 10, 827
3 of 17
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
A group of 34 female students from the University of Warmia and Mazury in Olsztyn participated
in this study. Females were selected over males because it is common in Poland that in the winter,
females spend their free time, as well as much of their time spent with children, on walks. For this
reason, we suggested that this form of activity related to staying in forest areas should be examined
by this experimental study involving females. Students were recruited from one study course at
the University (a non-forestry course), and participation in the study was voluntary. Two women
did not want to participate in the research voluntarily. Qualified participants (32 females; mean
age = 20.97 years, standard deviation (S.D.) = 0.65) were randomly assigned to one of two groups
(16 persons in each): The experimental group (Group 1) and the control group (Group 2). A simple
randomization method was used. To decide the appropriate sample size, the software G*Power 3.1
(University of Düsseldorf, Düsseldorf, Germany). was used. The eect size was set to 0.5, the alpha
error probability value was set to 0.05, and the power value was set to 0.95. It was found that the
total sample size should be at least 16. To ensure a satisfactory power value, the total sample size was
adjusted to 32.
Before the experiment, participants from each group were informed that they would be asked to
contribute to a research study of ‘forest recreation’ and informed consent was obtained. The purpose
of the study was explained thoroughly after the experiment, because the authors of the research
wished to omit the eect of suggestion on participants. All procedures performed in this study were in
accordance with the ethical standards of the Polish Committee of Ethics in Science and with the 1964
Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments.
2.2. Study Sites
Before the experiment, all participants completed research questionnaires under indoor conditions
in one of the classrooms at the University in Olsztyn city (north-eastern Poland). This place was also a
gathering point. Afterwards, the experiment was conducted in a forest environment at the forest point
(Group 1) and in an urban street environment at the urban point (Group 2). The participants reached
each of these two areas by walking. The locations of the indoor, forest, and urban street environments
are shown in Figure 1. The distance from the gathering point to either the urban point or the forest
point was approximately 1 km, which required 20 min of walking for participants in each group.
The roads to both points were constantly flat, without any hills or other hindrances.
The indoor environment was quite warm (21.5 C), exhibited no noise from outside, and did
not contain any potted plants. The forest environment was located at a 20 min walk from the
gathering point. A deciduous, broadleaved urban forest was selected, consisting of European beech
(Fagus sylvatica L.), Pedunculate oak (Quercus robur L.), Common aspen (Populus tremula L.), and Black
alder (Alnus glutinosa [L.] Gaertn.). Larger shrubs or dead wood did not interrupt the view in this area,
although in some places, Norway spruce (Picea abies [L.] H. Karst) was in the shrub layer. The ground
was mainly covered by snow during the experiment, and small amounts of snow were also visible
on trunks. The urban street environment (urban point) was also a 20 min walk from the gathering
point, near the center of Olsztyn. A particular place was selected without visible tree trunks, crowns,
and any greens. In some places in the city environment, the snow dissolved and was not visible during
the experiment (in the urban street environment, there was less snow in the landscape than in the
forest environment.) The forest environment and urban street environment used in the experiment are
shown in Figure 2.
Forests 2019, 10, 827
4 of 17
Figure 1. The map of experimental locations.
Figure 2. Photos showing the urban (A) and forest (B) views used for the viewing session. Photos of the
research group during viewing sessions in the urban street environment (C) and forest environment (D).
Forests 2019, 10, 827
5 of 17
During the field experiment, the level of noise was measured with the application ‘Sound level
analyzer (SLA)’ using an iPhone 6 (Apple Inc, Cupertino, CA, USA). This method of noise measure has
been scientifically tested and shown to give excellent results [20]. The mean sound level (± S.D.) in the
urban street environment was 66.61 ± 5.38 dB, whereas the mean sound level in the forest environment
was 37.18 ± 5.23 dB.
Meteorological data were not recorded in the forest and urban street environments; however,
information regarding temperature, humidity, and wind speed was available from the nearest area
recorded by a meteorological station. On the day of experiment, meteorological conditions were
noted from the meteorological station in Olsztyn-Mazury (location: 5328 50.0“ N, 2056 10.9“ E).
The temperature was 0.5 C, humidity was 100%, cloudiness was 100%, atmospheric pressure was
995 hPa, and the speed of the west wind was 9 km/h. No snow precipitation was observed, but snow
cover occurred on the ground and dissolved during the day in the city.
2.3. Psychological Measurements
In the current study, four psychological questionnaires were used to measure the reactions of
participants to the investigated environments. The Profile of Mood States (POMS) is a valid and
reliable measure of negative mood states, such as psychological distress, as well as positive mood
states, such as vigor [21]. This tool has been used previously to measure participants’ responses to
forest environments (e.g., Lee et al. [22]). POMS measures six dierent mood states: Tension/anxiety,
depression/dejection, anger/hostility, vigor, fatigue, and confusion. Occasionally, a short version of
this questionnaire is used. In this research, the regular Polish version with 65 items was applied [23].
The Positive and Negative Aect Schedule (PANAS) is another reliable and valid instrument for
measuring negative (10 items) and positive (10 items) aects [24]. Its original version is in English [25],
but the Polish edition was applied in the current study [26]. PANAS has also previously been used
for forest environment assessment (e.g., Takayama et al. [27]). The Restorative Outcome Scale (ROS),
which measures the restorative phenomenon, is a valid and reliable scale [28,29] that has been used
to measure restorativeness induced by the forest environment [27]. A Polish adaptation with six
items was successfully developed and used in this study [17]. The Subjective Vitality Scale (SVS),
which measures a participant’s level of vitality, is another scale that has been shown to be reliable and
valid [30]. The Polish adaptation, consisting of four items [17], was used in the current work.
In all four questionnaires that were used, the time frame ‘during present moment’ was applied,
which allowed for the measurement of participant reactions to dierent environments over a relatively
short time. All scales used in this study involved the Likert scale, with the response to each item noted
by participants as one of a continuous series of numbers. To assess items in the POMS scale, a 0–4
Likert scale was used. For PANAS, a 1–5 scale was used, and, for ROS and SVS, a 1–7 scale was used.
The raw data from each questionnaire were applied in all further calculations. The internal
consistencies and numbers of items for each scale and subscale are included in Table 1. Most of the
scales used exhibited good internal consistency, with the exception of the POMS subscale ‘confusion,’
where internal consistency was lower (but still acceptable).
2.4. Procedure
The meeting with participants was planned for 6 March 2018, at 9:00 am. At this time, while
at the gathering point, participants were randomly divided into one of two groups (experimental
and control). Each group then completed the questionnaires at the gathering point, a classroom at
the University. Afterwards, participants were asked to walk to the urban point or to the forest point,
with researchers to guide this walk (two researchers per group). At the destination point, participants
were asked to stand in a line, one meter apart from one another. Each participant was placed with
a proper view throughout the urban street or forest environment. The viewing property for each
participant was the same. While standing as described, participants were asked to relax and observe
the view for 15 min. Talking was not allowed. Relaxation during standing was applied because during
Forests 2019, 10, 827
6 of 17
the winter in Poland, it is too cold to relax in a seated position. People participating in the study were
healthy and fit. Subjects could not be under the influence of medicine (including alcohol) during the
examination. Before the test and directly during it, smoking and coee consumption as well as the
use of cell phones was prohibited. After 15 min of relaxation, participants were asked to fill out the
questionnaires once again. The procedure followed for the experiment is also described in Table 2.
Table 1. Verification of internal consistency and number of items for each (sub)scale.
Scales and Subscales Number of Items Cronbach’s Alpha
POMS
Tension/Anxiety
9
Depression/Dejection
15
Anger/Hostility
12
Vigor
8
Fatigue
7
Confusion
7
PANAS
Negative
10
Positive
10
ROS
6
SVS
4
0.854
0.891
0.867
0.829
0.883
0.794
0.881
0.832
0.921
0.807
POMS: Profile of Mood States; PANAS: Positive and Negative Aect Schedule; ROS: Restorative Outcome Scale;
SVS: Subjective Vitality Scale; n = 32.
Date
2018/03/06 (Thu.)
Table 2. Procedure followed during the experiment.
Time
09:00
09:15–09:30
09:30–09:45
09:45–10:05
10:05–10:10
10:10–10:25
10:25–10:40
10:40–11:05
11:05
Activity
Meeting in the gathering point and orientation
Random division into two groups
Group 1 (control)
Group 2 (forest)
Filling in questionnaires (pretest) Filling in questionnaires (pretest)
Walk to the city
Walk to the forest
Standing in a row in urban point Standing in a row in forest point
Standing and viewing at the urban
Standing and viewing at the
street environment
forest environment
Filling in questionnaires (posttest) Filling in questionnaires (posttest)
Return to the campus
Return to the campus
End of the experiment
2.5. Data Analysis
The means and S.D. values were calculated in Excel (Microsoft, Redmond, WA, USA). The distribution
of data was similar to the normal distribution, and this was checked by the Shapiro–Wilk test.
A parametric, mixed-design ANOVA was conducted to analyze the interactions and main eects of
the POMS, PANAS, ROS, and SVS scores as pre-post indicators of the psychological restorative eect
of exposure to the urban street versus forest environments. After ANOVA, post-hoc comparisons
using the Fisher’s least significant dierence (LSD) test were conducted, a method that has also been
used in previous studies [31,32]. For each analysis, eect size η2 was calculated, with eects set as:
Small = 0.10; medium = 0.30; and large = 0.50. All statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS
Statistics Version 24 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA).
3. Results
3.1. POMS
Two types of psychological restorative eects were considered: The eect of dierent conditions
(urban street vs. forest environment) and the eect of exposure to a dierent environment (pre vs. post).
Forests 2019, 10, 827
7 of 17
These two factors were analyzed using a mixed model ANOVA to compare the changes in the POMS
scores and to analyze the interactions between factors (Table 3). For four of the six POMS indicators
(tension/anxiety, depression/dejection, anger/hostility, confusion), interactions between conditions
and time were found. Regarding main eects, conditions had a significant eect on anger/hostility,
and time had a highly significant eect on fatigue.
The results of the Fisher’s least significant dierence (LSD) test comparisons showed that
tension/anxiety and anger/hostility were significantly increased after participants in the urban group
were exposed to the urban street environment (urban: Pre vs. post; Table 4). In contrast, tension/anxiety,
depression/dejection, anger/hostility, fatigue, and confusion significantly decreased in the forest group
after exposure to the forest environment (forest: Pre vs. post). All POMS indicators were similar in
both the urban and forest groups before the intervention (pre: Urban vs. forest). After the intervention,
all POMS indicators (except vigor) showed significantly lower values in the forest group than in the
urban group (post: Urban vs. forest).
Forests 2019, 10, 827
Table 3. Results for mixed-model ANOVAs investigating Profile of Mood States (POMS) scores.
Main Eect
Interaction
POMS
Conditions: Urban vs. Forest
F
p
η2
Time: Pre vs. Post
F
p
η2
Conditions × Time
F
p
η2
Tension/Anxiety
3.680 0.065 -
Depression/Dejection 0.881 0.355 -
Anger/Hostility
4.500 0.042 *
Vigor
0.023 0.879 -
Fatigue
2.587 0.118 -
Confusion
0.718 0.404 -
0.109 0.025 0.875
-
0.001 18.056 0.000 *** 0.376
0.029 0.386 0.539
-
0.013 7.315 0.011
*
0.196
0.130 1.185 0.285
-
0.038 16.198 0.000 *** 0.351
0.001 1.572 0.220
-
0.050 1.775 0.193
-
0.056
0.079 9.827 0.004 ** 0.247 3.263 0.081
-
0.098
0.023 0.273 0.605
-
0.009 9.172 0.005 ** 0.234
***: p < 0.001, **: p < 0.01, *p < 0.05, -: Not significant, mixed-design (split-plot) ANOVA; POMS: Profile of Mood States; n = 32.
Table 4. Results of multiple comparisons of POMS scores for urban versus forest environments, as well as before and after environmental exposure.
Urban
Forest
Pre
Post
Pre
Post
Average S.D. Average S.D.
p
Average S.D. Average S.D.
p
Tension/Anxiety
0.85
0.36
1.33
0.73
0.007
**
0.99
0.81
0.47
0.46
0.004
**
Depression/Dejection
0.71
0.56
0.95
0.55
0.151
-
0.85
0.72
0.48
0.54
0.025
*
Anger/Hostility
0.85
0.48
1.38
0.76
0.001
**
0.88
0.67
0.57
0.39
0.047
*
Vigor
1.93
0.78
1.91
0.71
0.956
-
1.75
0.72
2.15
0.73
0.077
-
Fatigue
1.81
0.88
1.59
0.84
0.355
-
1.65
1.13
0.82
0.91
0.002
**
Confusion
1.17
0.63
1.47
0.87
0.087
-
1.33
0.65
0.91
0.77
0.018
*
Pre
Post
Urban
Forest
Urban
Forest
Average S.D. Average S.D.
p
Average S.D. Average S.D.
p
Tension/Anxiety
0.85
0.36
0.99
0.81
0.522
-
Depression/Dejection
0.71
0.56
0.85
0.72
0.505
-
Anger/Hostility
0.85
0.48
0.88
0.67
0.902
-
Vigor
1.93
0.78
1.75
0.72
0.500
-
Fatigue
1.81
0.88
1.65
1.13
0.633
-
Confusion
1.17
0.63
1.33
0.65
0.541
-
1.33
0.73
0.47
0.46
0.000
***
0.95
0.55
0.48
0.54
0.029
*
1.38
0.76
0.57
0.39
0.000
***
1.91
0.71
2.15
0.73
0.360
-
1.59
0.84
0.82
0.91
0.026
*
1.47
0.87
0.91
0.77
0.039
*
***: p < 0.001, **: p < 0.01, *p < 0.05, -: Not significant, ANOVA-LSD test; n = 32, POMS: Profile of Mood States. S.D.: Standard Deviation.
8 of 17
Forests 2019, 10, 827
9 of 17
3.2. PANAS
A mixed-model ANOVA of the PANAS data was conducted, with conditions dierences and
time dierences used as two factors (Table 5). The results showed that an interaction occurred in the
case of ‘PANAS negative aect.’ Regarding main eects, no statistically significant dierences were
found for conditions or time. The results of the multiple comparisons tests (Table 6) showed that in the
case of the urban group, there was a marginal (p = 0.044) dierence in negative aect between the pre-
and post-tests. When comparing the urban and forest groups after the intervention (urban: Pre vs.
post), a marginal eect on ‘PANAS positive’ was observed (p = 0.033), with the positive aect slightly
increased in the forest environment; however, this was likely an eect of non-conservative comparison.
Table 5. Results for mixed-model ANOVAs investigating Positive and Negative Aect Schedule States
(PANAS) scores.
Main Eect
Interaction
PANAS
Conditions: Urban vs. Forest
F
p
η2
Time: Pre vs. Post
F
p
η2
Conditions × Time
F
p
η2
Negative 0.461 0.503 - 0.015 0.541 0.468 - 0.018 4.999 0.033 * 0.143
Positive 1.624 0.212 - 0.051 0.696 0.411 - 0.023 3.371 0.076 - 0.101
***: p < 0.001, **: p < 0.01, *: p < 0.05, -: Not significant, mixed-model ANOVA; n = 32, PANAS: Positive and Negative
Aect Schedule States.
Table 6. Results of multiple comparisons of PANAS scores for urban street versus forest environments,
as well as before and after environmental exposure.
Urban
Forest
Pre
Post
Pre
Post
Average S.D. Average S.D.
p
Average S.D. Average S.D.
p
PANAS Negative 1.43 0.42 1.79 0.64 0.044 * 1.59 0.72 1.41 0.51 0.297 -
PANAS Positive 2.73 0.69 2.56 0.74 0.484 - 2.65 0.63 3.08 0.62 0.069 -
Pre
Post
Urban
Forest
Urban
Forest
Average S.D. Average S.D.
p
Average S.D. Average S.D.
p
PANAS Negative 1.43 0.42 1.59 0.72 0.453 - 1.79 0.64 1.41 0.51 0.069 -
PANAS Positive 2.73 0.69 2.65 0.63 0.753 - 2.56 0.74 3.08 0.62 0.033 *
***: p < 0.001, **: p < 0.01, *p < 0.05, -: Not significant, ANOVA-LSD test; n = 32, PANAS: Positive and Negative
Aect Schedule; S.D.: Standard Deviation.
3.3. ROS
In the case of the ROS, a mixed-model ANOVA was used to investigate restorativeness of the two
environments, with conditions and time as factors. This analysis was similar to those conducted for data
from the POMS and PANAS questionnaires (Table 7). An interaction was observed between conditions
and time. Regarding main eects, a significant eect of environment was observed. The results of
multiple comparisons LSD tests (Table 8) showed that in the forest group, there was a significant
increase in ROS scores after exposure to the forest environment (forest: Pre vs. post). Furthermore,
in the forest group, values of ROS were higher after exposure to the forest environment than they were
in the urban group after exposure to the urban street environment (post: Urban vs. forest).
3.4. SVS
A mixed-model ANOVA was conducted to compare changes in SVS scores and to analyze the
interaction between factors and main eects, as was done for the POMS, PANAS, and ROS data
(Table 9). As with the ROS data, an interaction was observed between conditions and time for the SVS
score data. When the main eects of the conditions and time dierences were analyzed, a statistically
Forests 2019, 10, 827
10 of 17
significant eect of conditions was confirmed, and the time eect was not significant. The results of
LSD comparisons (Table 10) showed that in the forest group, SVS scores significantly increased after
exposure to the forest environment. Furthermore, after exposure to the forest environment, SVS scores
were higher than after exposure to the urban street environment.
Table 7. Results for mixed-model ANOVAs investigating Restorative Outcome Scale (ROS) scores.
Main Eect
Interaction
ROS
Conditions: Urban vs. Forest
F
p
η2
Time: Pre vs. Post
F
p
η2
Conditions × Time
F
p
η2
12.284 0.001 ** 0.291 0.220 0.643 - 0.007 8.885 0.006 ** 0.228
***: p < 0.001. **: p < 0.01. *: p < 0.05. -: Not significant; mixed-model ANOVA; n = 32; ROS: Restorative
Outcome Scale.
Table 8. Results of multiple comparisons of ROS scores for urban versus forest environments, as well
as before and after environmental exposure.
Urban
Forest
Pre
Post
Pre
Post
Average S.D. Average S.D.
p
Average S.D. Average S.D.
p
ROS 4.00 1.25 3.30 1.41 0.086 -
4.31 1.19 5.27 0.94 0.021 *
Pre
Post
Urban
Forest
Urban
Forest
Average S.D. Average S.D.
p
Average S.D. Average S.D.
p
ROS 4.00 1.25 4.31 1.19 0.468 -
3.30 1.41 5.27 0.94 0.000 ***
***: p < 0.001, **: p < 0.01, *: p < 0.05, -: Not significant, ANOVA-LSD test; n = 32, ROS: Restorative Outcome Scale;
S.D.: Standard Deviation.
Table 9. Results for mixed-model ANOVAs investigating Subjective Vitality Scale (SVS) scores.
Main Eect
Interaction
SVS
Conditions: Urban vs. Forest
F
p
η2
Time: Pre vs. Post
F
p
η2
Conditions × Time
F
p
η2
5.524 0.026 * 0.155 1.103 0.302 - 0.035 4.527 0.042 * 0.131
***: p < 0.001. **: p < 0.01. *: p < 0.05. -: Not significant; mixed-model ANOVA; n = 32; SVS: Subjective Vitality Scale.
Table 10. Results of multiple comparisons of SVS scores for urban street versus forest environments, as
well as before and after environmental exposure.
Urban
Forest
Pre
Post
Pre
Post
Average S.D. Average S.D.
p
Average S.D. Average S.D.
p
SVS 3.86 1.52 3.55 1.29 0.452 -
4.08 1.23 5.00 1.12 0.032 *
Pre
Post
Urban
Forest
Urban
Forest
Average S.D. Average S.D.
p
Average S.D. Average S.D.
p
SVS 3.86 1.52 4.08 1.23 0.635 -
3.55 1.29 5.00 1.12 0.002 **
***: p < 0.001, **: p < 0.01, *: p < 0.05, -: Not significant, ANOVA-LSD test; n = 32, SVS: Subjective Vitality Scale; S.D.:
Standard Deviation.
Forests 2019, 10, 827
11 of 17
4. Discussion
4.1. Mood States
Consistent with previous studies [2,5,10,11,14,17,33,34], this study confirmed that short periods
of forest recreation in a snow-covered forest (in this case: 15 min of relaxation in this environment)
have a significant eect on the mood states of participants. The negative indices of mood states,
including tension/anxiety, anger/hostility, depression/dejection, confusion, and fatigue, decreased after
exposure to the forest environment. This confirms our hypothesis that this type of intervention results
in a positive outcome. The positive index of mood states (vigor) did not increase after exposure,
but neither did it decrease significantly. This is not consistent with a previous study [14] or another
study conducted only among females [4]. Lower levels of indicators of negative mood have many
positive outcomes, confirming that brief forest recreation during the winter, when snow has occurred,
can be successfully conducted for the purpose of stress reduction among females. Females may,
however, react dierently to the forest environment than men [17], which indicates the need for further
testing of these eects on both sexes.
The eect of lowering negative mood states is useful information for therapists who work with
individuals living with high levels of stress, such as a highly stressful work environment. The positive
eect of nature therapy on this topic is already known [6–9], but a novel element of this study was the
addition of snow cover. As snow occurs in the winter in many countries, the way in which this element
aects individuals requires examination. A positive eect of forest recreation was still observed with
snow cover in this study, and there are several hypotheses as to why this eect was observed. It is
possible that snow in the forest environment does not obstruct the view that is generally visible in this
environment. Some authors suggest that some fractal dimensions are responsible for the existence
of the eect of visual stimulation on mood states. For example, some kinds of natural fractals might,
hypothetically, induce this positive eect [35,36], and in a snow-covered forest, these fractal dimensions
are still perceivable to respondents. This stimulation during forest recreation is crucial, and this
reaction of humans is possibly some special eect connected to the biophilia hypothesis [37], which
states that people evolved in a natural environment and hence feel healthy in a natural environment
like a forest [38].
4.2. Positive and Negative Aect
Previous studies have indicated that subjects exhibit a significant decrease in negative aect after
forest recreation and a significant increase in positive aect [17,27]. In the current study, an eect on
positive aect was observed, as this indicator increased in the forest environment. Negative aect
did not decrease in the forest environment after the experiment, however, although it did increase
after exposure to the urban street environment. These two indicators, positive and negative aect,
are important in psychological research, as their usage gives researchers and therapists information
concerning the mental state of participants and patients [24].
The interpretation of results regarding negative aect is similar to that of the negative mood
states of POMS—any negative symptoms are not necessary, so any decrease in this eect is welcome.
In this case, a lowering eect on this indicator was unfortunately not observed. In the urban street
environment, an increase in negative aect was observed, possibly due to the higher level of noise
in that environment [39]. Further research should compare environments with more similar levels
of noise. In the case of positive aect, a dierence was observed when comparing forest and urban
street environments. In the forest environment, this indicator had higher values, suggesting that
in the forest environment, successful recreation could be conducted with positive eects. Because
positive aect increased in the forest environment, this tendency might be explained by the biophilia
hypothesis [37,38].
Forests 2019, 10, 827
12 of 17
4.3. Restorative Outcome
Our findings are consistent with those of previous studies [17,27], showing that restorativeness
significantly increases after exposure to the forest environment. The phenomenon of restoration in the
natural environment has been previously described. This eect is frequently explained by attention
restoration theory [40], which states that the mental refreshment of humans, as measured by the level
of restoration, increases in natural environments. This theory suggests that this mental restoration
might be a natural tool of the human nervous system, one meant to naturally maintain alertness
in non-safe environments, such as the forest, as opposed to safe places, such as houses and caves.
Increasing thinking capacity in a natural environment could be a natural adaptation to hunting or
foraging. Perhaps restoration is a mechanism in humans predisposed to save calories [41], although
this hypothesis requires further investigation.
4.4. Subjective Vitality
Consistent with previous studies [17,27], this study’s findings suggest that short, 15 min periods
of relaxation in a snow-covered forest during winter can have psychological benefits for subjective
vitality levels. Vitality is a concept that can be measured objectively using physiological reactions or
subjectively using psychological reactions [30]. In the case of the current work, subjective methods were
used. The importance of factors that increase vitality is great, as a high level of vitality is connected with
better accomplishments of subjects [42], and anything that increases human performance is important
for society. Based on the current study, snow cover should not be a barrier to increasing vitality during
recreational stays or walks in the forest. Thus, successful forest recreation in snow-covered forests may
be a good activity for people interested in increasing their personal eectiveness by stimulating an
increase in their own vitality.
4.5. A Rigorous Comparison with the Previous Study
Previously, the same areas were used to study the impact of winter forest recreation (without
snow cover) on the psychological relaxation of the subjects [17]. It is worth referring to the results
of these studies in this manuscript. Particular attention should be paid to comparing the impact
of environments on psychological relaxation after conducting an experiment. In the case of the
POMS questionnaire results, current research showed a significant dierence between: Tension/anxiety
(p < 0.001), depression/dejection (p = 0.029), anger/hostility (p = 0.047), fatigue (p = 0.002) and
confusion (dierences for these two environments are irrelevant for the ‘vigor’ subscales). In the
previous study [17], the tension/anxiety subscale had lower values after exposure to the forest
environment compared to the urban street environment, similarly to the subscale depression/dejection,
anger/hostility, fatigue and confusion. The results for the ‘vigor’ subscales reached significantly higher
values. All dierences were compared at a significance level of p < 0.05. Comparing these two studies,
it can be seen that the only dierence was the significance for the vigor; the forest environment,
compared to urban street environment, therefore causes psychological relaxation regardless of the
presence or absence of snow cover, but this impact is somewhat smaller with snow cover.
In the case of the PANAS scale, the negative aspect in current research did not dier significantly
after exposure to two dierent environments (forest vs. urban street environment), but the positive
aspect was significantly higher after exposure to the forest environment (p = 0.033). In studies without
snow cover [17], the negative aspect was significantly lower in the forest environment and the positive
aspect was significantly higher in this environment (with a significance level of p < 0.05). Therefore,
the forest environment without snow cover had a slightly stronger impact on the subjects.
As for the ROS and SVS scales, the ROS scale value in the study was significantly higher in the
forest environment (p < 0.001), similarly to the SVS scale (p = 0.002). In the compared study [17], exactly
the same tendency was observed (with a significance level of p < 0.05), so the environments in both
studies had similar eects on the results obtained in the ROS and SVS scales.
Forests 2019, 10, 827
13 of 17
Focusing on the predominance of any of the analyzed variants (snow vs. no snow) is not easy,
because the demographics of the group and its numbers varied. This should be taken into consideration
when interpreting these results.
4.6. Limitations
This study has several limitations. First, in this study, the psychological eect of relaxation while
simply standing in the forest environment was measured. These eects should be measured during
other activities in a forest with snow cover. In fact, some reports indicate that being involved in certain
activities may harm the positive eects of nature relaxation [43]; it is therefore worth examining why
some activities induce relaxation and some do not.
Second, only females (20.97 ± 0.65 years) were involved in this study. Future studies should
investigate how recreation in a snow-covered forest influences females of dierent age groups, as well
as how this activity influences males of dierent ages [4,5].
Third, only psychological measurements were used in this case; the physiological eects of this
activity were not considered [44].
Fourth, dierent respondents went to each research site. The respondents at each research point
can therefore not be compared directly, meaning that the dierences among individuals in the two
respondent groups could have caused the significant dierences observed between the urban and
forest environments. In future studies, this occurrence could be eliminated by exposing respondents
from the two groups to both environments in reverse order [45].
Fifth, as respondents walked to each point on foot, they would have viewed surrounding scenery
before getting to either experimental point. It is therefore possible that the time period during which
respondents were exposed to the stimulus was not exactly 15 min in length. It is worth mentioning,
however, that the walks to the two analyzed environments were conducted through either the urban
street environment or the forest (Figure 1), so only one environment was able to influence each
group [46].
Sixth, in the planned experiments, a carryover eect could possibly have occurred, as the positive
eect of forest recreation on health can persist for a longer period of time in subjects, and the tests
had to be carried out as soon as possible (due to only periodic snowfall). Conducting a crossover
study could therefore be burdened with a carryover eect, and the use of this research system could be
abandoned. Thus, a parallel study could be carried out in which the carryover eect was avoided.
In addition, such a study can be carried out in a relatively short time. This study would not compare
the eects of forest recreation to the eects of conventional recreation (active control), which would
not take place under forest conditions. This is a limitation of the current research and should be
investigated in future work [46].
Seventh, in this study, it was assumed that snow was present in the forest landscape if 90% of
the ground’s surface was covered by it. It is not known, however, what amount of snow aects the
psychological reactions of subjects. This requires future research.
Eighth, in the described research, it was tested whether the forest environment with the existing
snow cover aects the psychological relaxation of the subjects. Indeed, it has been shown that in a
forest environment with a snow cover causes a psychological relaxation eect. The aim of the study
was not to compare the forest with the snow cover with the forest environment without snow cover;
however, in future studies, this type of comparison could be done (including the same participants) to
find out whether the snow cover in the forest environment aects another way on the subjects than the
forest environment without snow cover [18].
Ninth, it will also be important to examine how dierent seasons, during which forest stands in the
temperate climate zone change, can aect the intensity of the psychological relaxation of subjects [47].
Research regarding this should be carried out in the future.
Tenth, this paper focuses on examining the impact of a forest environment on the psychological
relaxation of young adult students. In the future, it is worth investigating how forest recreation, also
Forests 2019, 10, 827
14 of 17
conducted in winter, can aect people of dierent ages and from other demographic groups (e.g., older
people, and working).
Eleventh, this study did not include men, so future studies concerning recreation in a forest
environment with snow cover present should include this group [5,11].
Twelfth, it is also possible that spending too much time in the forest may have a negative eect,
depending on the temperature. For this reason, encouraging people to move around to stay warm may
be less stressful. The eect of ambient temperature should be examined in future research [47]. It is
worth noting that the physical sensations may have been dierent in the urban street environment
than in the forest environment. This study did not measure these feelings in this experiment. It would
be useful in further studies to include a measurement of physical sensations and possibly control the
positioning of experimental fields to eliminate dierences between environments. It is worth adding,
however, that the day of the experiment was chosen so that the wind intensity was as low as possible
and the dierences between the environments were as small as possible.
Thirteenth, in the experiment, there was less snow in the urban street environment, but it
was noticeable in this landscape. In the experiment, it was important to check whether the forest
landscape with snow actually aected psychological relaxation—research confirms that this is the
case. The intensity of snow occurrence in the urban landscape (in the control environment) is less
important [47].
Fourteenth, the impact of phytoncides from trees growing in the forest may also be significant
and are worth investigating in future studies. It is worth emphasizing that in winter, this eect may
not occur (lack of photosynthesis) in comparison to forest areas in spring and summer [48].
All these limitations could likely be overcome in further experiments in the area of forest recreation
research. Future studies should also test the eect of forest recreation on males and on dierent age
groups (e.g., on elderly participants).
5. Conclusions
This study examined the eect of a snow-covered forest environment during winter on young
females’ psychological relaxation, with the urban street environment as a control. The results
showed that participants’ levels of negative mood indicators (tension/anxiety, anger/hostility,
depression/dejection, confusion, and fatigue) decreased after exposure to a forest environment
with snow cover. Furthermore, some of these indicators increased in the urban street environment
(tension/anxiety and anger/hostility). An indicator of negative aect increased after exposure to the
urban street environment, whereas an indicator of positive aect was higher in the forest environment
than in the urban street environment. Restorativeness and subjective vitality exhibited higher values
after exposure to the forest environment in comparison to both the control and pre-test. This indicates
that forest recreation, during winter and with snow cover, continues to have a significant influence
on the psychological relaxation of young females. These findings are important for forest therapy
practitioners, as well as for individuals who want to obtain the positive eects of forest recreation.
Such recreation could be successfully conducted during winter in a forest with snow cover, and there
should still be a positive eect on psychological parameters.
Author Contributions: E.B. conceived and designed the experiment, conducted data analysis, and prepared the
first version of the manuscript. A.Ł. consulted on the design of the experiment, and reviewed and edited the
manuscript. A.O. and S.B. were responsible for the implementation of field research and minor editorial comments
on the text. N.T. contributed to publication by reviewing and editing the manuscript. D.L.G. contributed to
publication by reviewing and editing the manuscript and also contributed to publication by editing the content
and English of the manuscript.
Funding: The publication process was supported by funds of the Poznan´ University of Life Sciences.
The publication is co-financed within the framework of Ministry of Science and Higher Education program as
“Regional Initiative Excellence” in years 2019-2022, project number 005/RID/2018/19.
Acknowledgments: The authors of this article thank Dominik Dobrzyn´ ski for technical assistance during
the research.
Forests 2019, 10, 827
15 of 17
Conflicts of Interest: The authors declare no conflicts of interest.
References
1. Douglas, R.W. Forest Recreation, 3rd ed.; Pergamon Press: New York, NY, USA, 1982; p. 336.
2. Lee, J.; Tsunetsugu, Y.; Takayama, N.; Park, B.J.; Li, Q.; Song, C.; Komatsu, M.; Ikei, H.; Tyrväinen, L.;
Kagawa, T.; et al. Influence of forest therapy on cardiovascular relaxation in young adults. Evid.-Based
Compl. Alt. 2014, 834360, 1–7. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
3. Sung, J.; Woo, J.M.; Kim, W.; Lim, S.K.; Chung, E.J. The eect of cognitive behavior therapy-based “forest
therapy” program on blood pressure, salivary cortisol level, and quality of life in elderly hypertensive
patients. Clin. Exp. Hypertens. 2012, 34, 1–7. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
4. Ochiai, H.; Ikei, H.; Song, C.; Kobayashi, M.; Miura, T.; Kagawa, T.; Li, Q.; Kumeda, S.; Imai, M.; Miyazaki, Y.
Physiological and psychological eects of a forest therapy program on middle-aged females. Int. J. Environ.
Res. Public Health 2015, 12, 15222–15232. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
5. Ochiai, H.; Ikei, H.; Song, C.; Kobayashi, M.; Takamatsu, A.; Miura, T.; Kagawa, T.; Li, Q.; Kumeda, S.;
Imai, M.; et al. Physiological and psychological eects of forest therapy on middle-aged males with
high-normal blood pressure. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2015, 12, 2532–2542. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
6. Morita, E.; Fukuda, S.; Nagano, J.; Hamajima, N.; Yamamoto, H.; Iwai, Y.; Nakashima, T.; Ohira, H.;
Shirakawa, T. Psychological eects of forest environments on healthy adults: Shinrin-yoku (forest-air bathing,
walking) as a possible method of stress reduction. Public Health 2007, 121, 54–63. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
7. Jung, W.H.; Woo, J.M.; Ryu, J.S. Eect of a forest therapy program and the forest environment on female
workers’ stress. Urban For. Urban Gree. 2015, 14, 274–281. [CrossRef]
8. Poulsen, D.V.; Stigsdotter, U.K.; Djernis, D.; Sidenius, U. ‘Everything just seems much more right in nature’:
How veterans with post-traumatic stress disorder experience nature-based activities in a forest therapy
garden. Health Psychol. Open 2016, 3, 1–14. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
9. Tyrväinen, L.; Ojala, A.; Korpela, K.; Lanki, T.; Tsunetsugu, Y.; Kagawa, T. The influence of urban green
environments on stress relief measures: A field experiment. J. Environ. Psychol. 2014, 38, 1–9. [CrossRef]
10. Tsunetsugu, Y.; Park, B.J.; Lee, J.; Kagawa, T.; Miyazaki, Y. Psychological relaxation eect of forest therapy:
Results of field experiments in 19 forests in Japan involving 228 participants. Jap. J. Hyg. 2011, 66, 670–676.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]
11. Song, C.; Joung, D.; Ikei, H.; Igarashi, M.; Aga, M.; Park, B.J.; Miwa, M.; Takagaki, M.; Miyazaki, Y.
Physiological and psychological eects of walking on young males in urban parks in winter.
J. Physiol. Anthrop. 2013, 32, 1–18. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
12. Song, C.; Ikei, H.; Miyazaki, Y. Physiological eects of nature therapy: A review of the research in Japan.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2016, 13, 1–17. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
13. Kamioka, H.; Tsutani, K.; Mutoh, Y.; Honda, T.; Shiozawa, N.; Okada, S.; Park, S.J.; Kitayuguchi, J.;
Kamada, M.; Okuizumi, H.; et al. A systematic review of randomized controlled trials on curative and health
enhancement eects of forest therapy. Psych. Res. Behav. Manag. 2012, 5, 85–95. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
14. Li, Q.; Kawada, T. Eect of forest therapy on the human psycho-neuro-endocrino-immune network.
Jap. J. Hyg. 2011, 66, 645–650. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
15. Hanai, T. Tourism meets psychology. J. Glob. Tour. Res. 2016, 1, 89–92.
16. Sidenius, U.; Stigsdotter, U.K.; Dahl Refshauge, A. A year in the therapy forest garden Nacadia®-participants’
use and preferred locations in the garden during a nature-based treatment program. Alam Cipta 2015, 8,
44–53.
17. Bielinis, E.; Takayama, N.; Boiko, S.; Omelan, A.; Bielinis, L. The eect of winter forest bathing on psychological
relaxation of young Polish adults. Urban For. Urban Gree. 2018, 29, 276–283. [CrossRef]
18. Tyrväinen, L.; Silvennoinen, H.; Hallikainen, V. Eect of the season and forest management on the visual
quality of the nature-based tourism environment: A case from Finnish Lapland. Scand. J. For. Res. 2017, 32,
349–359. [CrossRef]
19. Choi, J.Y.; Park, S.A.; Jung, S.J.; Lee, J.Y.; Son, K.C.; An, Y.J.; Lee, S.W. Physiological and psychological
responses of humans to the index of greenness of an interior space. Complement. Ther. Med. 2016, 28, 37–43.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]
Forests 2019, 10, 827
16 of 17
20. Murphy, E.; King, E.A. Testing the accuracy of smartphones and sound level meter applications for
measurement environmental noise. Appl. Acoust. 2016, 106, 16–22. [CrossRef]
21. McNair, D.M.; Maurice, L. An analysis of mood in neurotics. J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 1964, 69, 620–627.
[CrossRef]
22. Lee, J.; Park, B.J.; Tsunetsugu, Y.; Ohira, T.; Kagawa, T.; Miyazaki, Y. Eect of forest bathing on physiological
and psychological responses in young Japanese male subjects. Public Health 2011, 125, 93–100. [CrossRef]
[PubMed]
23. Dudek, B.; Koniarek, J. The adaptation of Profile of Mood States (POMS) by D.M. McNair, M. Lorr and L.F.
Droppelman. Przegla˛d Psychologiczny 1987, 30, 753–762. (in Polish).
24. Crawford, J.R.; Henry, J.D. The Positive and Negative Aect Schedule (PANAS): Construct validity,
measurement properties and normative data in a large non-clinical sample. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 2004, 43,
245–265. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
25. Watson, D.; Clark, L.A.; Tellegen, A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative
aect: The PANAS scales. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1998, 54, 1063. [CrossRef]
26. Brzozowski, P. Polish Adaptation of the Positive and Negative Aect Scale (PANAS) by David Watson and Lee Anna
Clark; PTPPTP: Warsaw, Poland, 2010; pp. 34–113. (in Polish)
27. Takayama, N.; Korpela, K.; Lee, J.; Morikawa, T.; Tsunetsugu, Y.; Park, B.J.; Li, Q.; Tyrväinen, L.; Miyazaki, Y.;
Kagawa, T. Emotional, restorative and vitalizing eects of forest and urban environments at four sites in
Japan. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2014, 11, 7207–7230. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
28. Korpela, K.M.; Ylén, M.; Tyrväinen, L.; Silvennoinen, H. Determinants of restorative experiences in everyday
favorite places. Health Place 2008, 14, 636–652. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
29. Korpela, K.M.; Ylén, M.; Tyrväinen, L.; Silvennoinen, H. Favorite green, waterside and urban environments:
Restorative experiences and perceived health in Finland. Health Promot. Int. 2010, 25, 200–209. [CrossRef]
[PubMed]
30. Ryan, R.M.; Frederick, C. On energy, personality, and health: Subjective vitality as a dynamic reflection of
well-being. J. Pers. 1997, 65, 529–565. [CrossRef]
31. Pedersen, E.; Weisner, S.E.; Johansson, M. Wetland areas’ direct contributions to residents’ well-being entitle
them to high cultural ecosystem values. Sci. Total Environ. 2019, 646, 1315–1326. [CrossRef]
32. Dobbie, M.F. Public aesthetic preferences to inform sustainable wetland management in Victoria, Australia.
Landscape Urban Plan. 2013, 120, 178–189. [CrossRef]
33. Ikei, H.; Song, C.; Kagawa, T.; Miyazaki, Y. Physiological and psychological eects of viewing forest
landscapes in a seated position in one-day forest therapy experimental model. Jap. J. Hyg. 2014, 69, 104–110.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]
34. Joung, D.; Kim, G.; Choi, Y.; Lim, H.; Park, S.; Woo, J.M.; Park, B.J. The prefrontal cortex activity and
psychological eects of viewing forest landscapes in autumn season. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health
2015, 12, 7235–7243. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
35. Kaiserman, J.G. Urban Nature for Well-being: Design Recommendations for Psychological Benefits in Urban
Public Spaces. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Washington, Seattle, WA, USA, 2017.
36. Van Almkerk, M.; Huisman, G. Virtual Nature Environments Based on Fractal Geometry for Optimizing
Restorative Eects. Environment 2018, 12, 1–11.
37. Wilson, E.O. Biophilia: The Human Bond with Other Species; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1984;
p. 160.
38. Kobayashi, H.; Song, C.; Ikei, H.; Kagawa, T.; Miyazaki, Y. Analysis of individual variations in autonomic
responses to urban and forest environments. Evid.-Based Compl. Alt. 2015, 2015, 1–7. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
39. Benfield, J.A.; Bell, P.A.; Troup, L.J.; Soderstrom, N.C. Aesthetic and aective eects of vocal and trac noise
on natural landscape assessment. J. Environ. Psychol. 2010, 30, 103–111. [CrossRef]
40. Kaplan, S. The restorative benefits of nature: Toward an integrative framework. J. Environ. Psychol. 1995, 15,
169–182. [CrossRef]
41. Cheval, B.; Tipura, E.; Burra, N.; Frossard, J.; Chanal, J.; Orsholits, D.; Radel, R.; Boisgontier, M.P. Avoiding
sedentary behaviors requires more cortical resources than avoiding physical activity: An EEG study.
Neuropsychologia 2018, 119, 68–80. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
42. Hone, L.C.; Jarden, A.; Schofield, G.M.; Duncan, S. Measuring flourishing: The impact of operational
definitions on the prevalence of high levels of wellbeing. Int. J. Wellbeing 2014, 4, 62–90. [CrossRef]
Forests 2019, 10, 827
17 of 17
43. Jiang, B.; Schmillen, R.; Sullivan, W.C. How to waste a break: Using portable electronic devices substantially
counteracts attention enhancement eects of green spaces. Environ. Beh. 2018, 51, 1–28. [CrossRef]
44. Bielinis, E.; Bielinis, L.; Krupin´ ska-Szeluga, S.; Łukowski, A.; Takayama, N. The Eects of a Short Forest
Recreation Program on Physiological and Psychological Relaxation in Young Polish Adults. Forests 2019,
10, 34. [CrossRef]
45. Saito, H.; Horiuchi, M.; Takayama, N.; Fujiwara, A. Eects of managed forest versus unmanaged forest on
physiological restoration from a stress stimulus, and the relationship with individual traits. J. Forest Res.
2019, 24, 77–85. [CrossRef]
46. Takayama, N.; Saito, H.; Fujiwara, A.; Horiuchi, M. The eect of slight thinning of managed coniferous forest
on landscape appreciation and psychological restoration. Progr. Earth Planet. Sci. 2017, 4, 17. [CrossRef]
47. Bielinis, E.; Omelan, A.; Boiko, S.; Bielinis, L. The restorative eect of staying in a broad-leaved forest on
healthy young adults in winter and spring. Balt. For. 2018, 24, 218–227.
48. Nam, E.S.; Uhm, D.C. Eects of phytoncides inhalation on serum cortisol level and life stress of college
students. Korean J. Adult Nurs. 2008, 20, 697–706.
© 2019 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access
article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution
(CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).