ISSUES OF ENVIRONMENTAL (IR)RESPONSIBILITY
ACTA UNIVERSITATIS AGRICULTURAE ET SILVICULTURAE MENDELIANAE BRUNENSIS
Volume LXI
230
http://dx.doi.org/10.11118/actaun201361072085
Number 7, 2013
ISSUES OF ENVIRONMENTAL
(IR)RESPONSIBILITY
Jana Dundelová
Received: April 11, 2013
Abstract
DUNDELOVÁ JANA: Issues of environmental (ir)responsibility. Acta Universitatis Agriculturae
et Silviculturae Mendelianae Brunensis, 2013, LXI, No. 7, pp. 2085–2092
In this paper the author reflects the questions of humans’ relationship to the environment and of
their ecological behaviour that becomes together with the development of modern technologies
increasingly relevant just as the question whether humans are able to affect fundamentally the
environment on the Earth by their activities. According to some authors (e.g. Ehrlich, 1968; Gore, 2006;
Wilson, 1995; Winter, Koger, 2009; Šmajs, 2005) human survival is directly connected with people’s
relationship to the nature; but other influential authors have contradictory opinions or they are at
least afraid of overestimation of ecological activities that can lead to neglecting of other important
problems (e.g. Simon, 1981; Goklany, 2007; Lomborg, 2007; Klaus, 2007, 2009).
These issues are dealt in this article mainly from the perspective of psychological theories and
concepts – the Freud’s concept of unconsciousness is discussed as well as groupthink, theory of
dissonance, contingency trap, Milgram’s theory of autonomous and agentic state of consciousness,
group and intergroup behaviour, social dilemma – tragedy of the commons.
Achieving a sustainable way of life depends on the equilibrium between consumption of individuals
and regenerative abilities of the natural environment. However, people still behave as if they were
separated from the nature. The linking axis of this article is the question of psychic powers causing
individual and collective ecological (ir)responsibility and the resulting consequences.
environmentalism, ecopsychology, paradigm, sustainable development, pseudo-ecology, global
solution, responsibility
1 AIM AND METHODS
The aim of this paper is a critical evaluation
of relation of human to its environment, to the
nature. This issue can be viewed from many
perspectives – ecological, environmentalistic,
political, economic, philosophical, psychological
etc. In this article psychological aspects of this
problem are highlighted, but elaborating this text
I realized that they cannot be solved separately,
that is why in the opening part of this study
important terms and concepts are defined as well
as the neverending dilemma of human ability to
influence fundamentally the changes in the nature
is mentioned here. The core of this paper is devoted
to psychological aspects that cause that some people
have tendency to environmentally protective
behaviour while the others feel to be separated
from nature or even to be superior to it. These
questions are set into the political and economical
frame and behind the lines is emphasized that for
understanding human behaviour to the nature is
required more than the knowledge of separated
disciplines.
This paper was based mainly on analysis of written
sources using a comparative approach enriched by
the author’s notes and comments.
2 DEFINITION OF BASIC TERMS AND
CONCEPTS
To be able to speak about issues of environmental
responsibility or irresponsibility we have to mention
basic terms, concepts and paradigms that create
platform for – in many times different – attitudes
and conclusions.
2085
2086
Jana Dundelová
Environmentalism1 is a philosophy, ideology,
social and political movement whose central topic is
the relationship of humankind to the environment.
Environmentalism strives to change the social,
political and economic mechanisms that damage
the environment. The origins of environmentalism
as a political theory were set in the 60th of 20th
century, when scientists and populists began to
point to an impending population explosion.
Environmentalism o en comes into opinion
conflict with proponents of freedom as basic
value of humans. So we can see on the one hand
o en very suggestive efforts drawing attention to
a deteriorating quality of the environment due
to human activities and to proposals of various
ecological arrangements, and on the other hand
endeavours emphasizing human freedom and
the danger of misuse of environmental activities
for economic and political manipulation with
consequences of limitation of human freedom.
The representative of the first approach is one of
the founders of environmentalism Paul Ehrlich, who
in his book “The Population Bomb” 2 (Ehrlich, 1968)
warned of a population explosion and predicted that
in the 70th of 20th century massive starvation in the
world will start and millions of people die of hunger.
(Of course the failure of this prediction was later
attacked by his opponents, including Václav Klaus).
Another important representative of ecological
activities is former candidate for U.S. president – Al
Gore – who in his book “An Inconvenient Truth”
(Gore, 2006) and in his presentations suggestively
warns against “the worst disaster in human history”
– which is undoubtedly meant an ecological disaster
mainly caused by global warming. 3
Environmental activities have their support even
in philosophical works, for example Šmajs (2005)
puts into contradiction “natural evolution” that
created the universe and “the evolution of anti-
natural human culture” – this ontic conflict of
culture and nature is, according to Šmajs, the deepest
essence of the current ecological crisis. “People
uncritically admire culture, and on the contrary,
they consider the nature to be useless, boring and
uninteresting” Šmajs (2005, p.7). “But the ‘romantic’
idea of nature as infinitely loving mother that all
human activities willingly accepts and endures,
and does not punish them evilly is unsustainable.
Development of cultural being is aimed against the
creativity of natural evolution; this development
destroys the most precious values of nature and
causes impairment of values of natural being”
Šmajs (2005, p. 11). These values of natural being
represented by biological diversity (biodiversity),
which is now reduced the most quickly since the
natural disasters at the end of the Mesozoic, which
killed the dinosaurs (Wilson, 1995).
Contradictorily, Klaus (see below) assigns an
insignificant value to biodiversity (and thus also
to its reduction) and considers also the question
of values; according to him they are likewise
biodiversity indefinable without human subject and
they have no sense – the man attributes the sense
to all values (Klaus, 2007, 2009). Klaus and other
authors with similar paradigm are afraid of abuse
“conservation” for promoting of various political
and economic arrangements. Klaus in his book
“Blue, not Green Planet” points out the possibility
of this negative impact already in the subtitle of the
book: “What is Endangered: Climate or Freedom?”,
and he highlights the lack of scientific evidence
confirming the influence of human activities on
global warming.
The polemic issue is also the depletion of
exhaustible natural resources (which are non-
renewable) and from the point of view of the
anti-environmentalists these resources are only
potential because the meaning, respectively
the usability, is given to them only the human
and their technology that is constantly evolving
and with this development “new” resources are
“discovered”. This idea is elaborated in Simon’s
book “The Ultimate Resource” (Simon, 1981) where
the ultimate resource is represented by human and
their ability to transform potential resources into
real. Similarly, a few years ago mass media published
a statement of Sheikh Zaki Yamani4 (which is an
evident paraphrase of the ideas of Goklany, 2007
and Lomborg, 2007; author’s note): “The Stone Age
did not end for lack of stone, and the Oil Age will
end long before the world runs out of oil” (www.
economist.com).5
Also Lomborg (2007) in his book “The Sceptical
Environmentalist” says that things are getting
better, not worse. The life expectancy increases,
infant mortality declines and the prosperity rises.
1 Environmentalism emphasizes the preservation, restoration and/or improvement of the natural environment, and
may be called as a movement to control pollution. Concepts such as a land ethic, environmental ethics, biodiversity,
ecology and the biophilia hypothesis (Wilson, 1984) are relevant in this context. /author’s note/
2 Paul R. Ehrlich wrote this book together with his wife, Anne Ehrlich, but she is quoted as a co-author in later editions.
/author’s note/
3 Global warming (and the depletion of nonrenewable resources) has become the most topical issue of
environmentalists and ecologists currently. /author’s note/
4 Ahmed Zaki Yamani (born 1930) is a Saudi Arabian politician who was Minister of Oil (Petroleum) and Mineral
Resources from 1962 to 1986, and a minister in OPEC (Organization of the Petroleum Exporting Countries) for 25
years. /author’s note/
5 However, if the abovementioned authors are wrong and people do not invented new technologies using “new” natural
resources to replace those depleted ones, it will be difficult to find a consolation in these theories. /author’s note/
Issues of environmental (ir)responsibility
2087
Lomborg’s main argument is that vast majority
of environmental problems such as pollution,
water shortages, deforestation, and species loss as
well as population growth, hunger, and AIDS, are
area-specific and highly correlated with poverty.
Therefore, challenges to human prosperity are
essentially logistical matters, and can be solved
largely through economic and social development.
Concerning problems that are more pressing at the
global level, such as the depletion of fossil fuels
and global warming, Lomborg argues that these
issues are o en overstated and that recommended
policies are o en inappropriate if assessed against
alternatives.
Lomborg argues that scientists – conservationists,
environmentalists and ecologists – work on
the assumption based on exaggerated and
sometimes even falsified information about global
environmental problems, which is exactly the same
accusation that these scientists ascribe to Lomborg
(Winter, Koger, 2009).
We can see that the environmental activities
are viewed from two totally different paradigms
and the aim of this paper is not to find arguments
in favour of one or the other, but to consider the
psychological phenomena causing that humans like
predators fundamentally change the environment
and with this attempt their own survival tries to
ignore the nature or solve the ecological problems
only utilitaristicly with the effort to overshadow the
economic or political profit of these activities.
3 ECOLOGICAL BEHAVIOUR IN
PSYCHOLOGICAL CONTEXT
The theme of the nature and the environment
becomes an important issue for psychology in the
early twenties of the last century. First, psychologists
focused more on the impact of environment on
humans (J. B. Watson, K. Lewin, E. Brunswik).
Environmentally oriented researchers recognized
the need to explore the relationship between
environmental stimuli and human reactions, and
use this knowledge to solve current problems
(Výrost, Slaměník, 1998).
Thus, the discipline, which is called
environmental psychology, is established
and at that time it is defined as a study of the
influence of environment on the human psyche.
Since the early seventies of the 20th century the
environmental movement has been formed and the
issues related to the protection of the environment
also began to be asked by some psychologists –
gradually “ecological psychology”, “conservation
psychology”, “psychology of environmental
problems” and “psychology of sustainable
development” came into existence. For all these
disciplines there are common themes expressing
human’s relationship to the nature.
For this relationship between human and
nature we can find two different approaches in the
literature:
a) the human is an exogenous factor for the nature;
which means that the human is an external factor
for the nature and is able to change it (to destroy,
“to save” it etc.);
b) the human an endogenous factor in relation
to the nature, i.e. the human is a part of the
nature (which however in my opinion cannot be
a paradigm for excuses of all bad human deeds
and actions in their relation to the nature).
As we can see both these approaches can be
used within the ecological efforts or misused in
the context of “pseudoecological” activities, in
which beautiful speeches about the protection of
nature only hide economic and political objectives.
Unfortunately, it is very difficult to distinguish one
from the other not only for the concerned ones
but especially for bystanders. Moreover, even good
intentions may be based on false premises; nothing
to say about the power of our unconsciousness (see
below).
If we look at the human psyche from the
perspective of psychoanalysis, we can see that
under the visible part of the imaginary iceberg
illustrating our personality a much larger part is
hidden, i.e. our unconscious, which can sometimes
give unexpected turn to our decisions and actions. In
the context of relation human – environment would
be appropriate to underline here Freud’s concept of
eros versus thanatos.6 The terms eros and thanatos
originally come from Greek mythology and Freud
used these mythical figures for description of the
life and death drives that co-exist within the psyche.
Eros represents the sexual drive, life, creative forces,
growth, productivity, construction and increase
of tension; eros inspires us to strive for individual
happiness and realize our wishes. It drives living
organisms to develop. Thanatos represents the
effort to eliminate all tensions, and it heads for
dissolution, negation, destructiveness and death.
Thanatos drives us toward a return to the inorganic.
We are constantly stimulated and driven into action
by a balance of these energies. Both fundamental
drives are empowered by libido energy. According
to Freud, these two forces fight each other, and their
conflict and interaction determine the development
of individual life and culture.
For one of the options how to explain the
destructive tendencies that are concealed in humans
Freud’s concept of thanatos can be used, which is in
6 The terms Eros and Thanatos originally come from Greek mythology. Eros was the God of love and desire; it has been
argued that Eros has two possible origins: the first theory states that Eros was the son of the Goddess Aphrodite. The
second suggests that “Eros emerged from Chaos…along with Gaia (the Earth) and Tartarus (the Underword).” Thanatos,
in contrast to Eros, was the Greek God of death and the son of Nyx (the Goddess of night).
2088
Jana Dundelová
this theory inextricably linked with eros, that is (or)
should be a creative force, but humans’ creativity
is a problematic aspect for the nature even if it is
associated with the best intentions, without the
intervention of unconscious destructive forces.
Positives of eros can be seen in the source of our
philosophic or religious explorations and in the
urge for self-actualization provided it is connected
with positive ethical values.
Humans are social creatures and carry out most
of their activities together with other individuals
who may ultimately affect their decisions. The
psychological literature o en mentions the term
groupthink, which is related mainly to political
causes, but it can be applied to group decisions
with environmental impacts. The principle of this
phenomenon lies in the fact that the group is closed
to relevant and objective information from the
outside and consequently takes decisions which
catastrophically fail.
The illusion of invulnerability is one of
symptoms of groupthink. It is connected with
excessive optimism that encourages members of the
group to take extreme risks. So people overestimate
their „immunity“ to threats in many situations. This
symptom is interconnected with rationalization.
The classic example o en cited in the literature is
a crisis in the Bay of Pigs (1962). A er this military
fiasco J. F. Kennedy (the president of U.S.A. that time)
took action that led to the new decision strategy:
supporting free expression of ideas, openness to
information, the occasional absence of authority
(the president) in the decision making process, the
role of “devil’s advocate” (i.e. the opponent or critic
of the authority). In the case of environmental
management decisions should be incorporated the
role of “devil’s advocate”, i.e. of a critic who should
defend environmental interests (which are however
o en in a discord with the economic interests of the
company).
Similar phenomena like groupthink are
obedience and conformity. Obedience is primarily
known from experiments of Milgram (1963)
and Hofling (1966 in Hayes, 2007), when most of
the participants were willing to obey at the first
sight unreasonable order of authority even if it
was contrary to their conscience. Also studies of
conformity (Sherif, 1935, Asch 1951 in Hewstone,
Stroebe 2006) show how difficult it is to resist
authority, or more precisely in these cases, to group
pressure and keep own opinion. For environmental
or ecological education is a positive finding that the
mere knowledge of these experiments reduced the
conformity of the research participants in some
subsequent experiments; and thus we can assume
that training of resisting the group pressure should
have positive results. But the question remains
whether the corporate culture will sometimes
reach such an eco-ethical level to be interested
in education and training of their employees
in resisting the pressure of the group, pointless
conformity or obedience, and if environmental/
ecological aspects will be sometimes superior to
economic aims and, if we do not get rather stuck in
what Baum (1994) called “contingency trap” – that is
a concept similar to the “social trap” – and expresses
the difficulty to change our bad habits. We will not
give up driving, using cosmetics tested on animals,
etc. because we are lazy, we are accustomed to do it
and it is convenient for us.
This issue of human selfishness and ruthlessness
and its impact on the environment is elaborated in
the concept “the Tragedy of the Commons” (Hardin,
1968). The tragedy of the commons represents
the depletion of a shared resource by individuals,
acting independently and rationally according to
man’s self-interest, despite their understanding that
depleting of the common resource is contrary to
the group’s long-term best interests. The tragedy of
the commons occurs when individuals neglect the
well-being of society (or the group) in the pursuit of
personal gain.
“The tragedy of the commons” is frequently
cited as a consequence for policies which restrict
private property and espouse expansion of public
property. Hardin’s proposals how to solve this
tragedy emphasize private property put an accent
on regulatory measures7 (according to Hardin even
freedom in the commons brings a ruin to all).
Both these proposals (private property and/
or regulations), however, have their weaknesses.
We can be witnesses how various responsibility
took the new owners to their possessions acquired
in restitution, we can see that private ownership
does not always correlate positively with the
responsibility to this object, but that the desire
for profit can on the contrary to devastate it (e.g.
urban development on fertile soil, environmentally
unsecured dump sites, junk yards in inappropriate
places). And if we look at regulatory measures,
we would get back into the vicious circle of
bureaucracy, corruption that products usefulness
but in reality useless measures.8 Therefore,
from the psychological point of view the issue of
responsibility and conscience is further mentioned.
One of the main outcomes of famous Milgram’s
experiment with electroshocks (Milgram, 1963)9
was the creation of agency theory in which Milgram
7 Legal aspects of this issue considers e.g. Jurčík (2007).
8 The question: what are the real environmental concerns, and what are mere meaningless measures, whose goals are
only political and/or economic is a topic for endless debate. /author’s note/
9 Stanley Milgram was interested in how easily ordinary people could be influenced into committing atrocities for
example, Germans in WWII.Milgram (1963) wanted to investigate whether Germans were particularly obedient to
authority figures as this was a common explanation for the Nazi killings in World War II. He examined justifications
Issues of environmental (ir)responsibility
2089
suggested two states in which people operate – the
autonomous and agentic. In autonomous state
individuals make decisions based on their own
ideas, beliefs and experiences. In agentic state
individuals give up their own responsibility, even
if they had to act in contrary to their own values,
and they only obey superiors. When people shi
from the autonomous state to the agentic state (the
agentic shi ) they give up their responsibility and
follow orders without considering adequacy and
the consequences of the request. This diffusion
of responsibility means that the person no longer
monitors their own behaviour. Milgram believed
that his participants were ‘just following orders’
and did not consider themselves to be responsible;
his participants even sighed with relief when the
experimenter said: “I am responsible for what
happens here.”
Agency theory also explains why some
people do not obey – they have remained in the
autonomous state (or they shi back to it from
agentic state) where they are able to make informed
decisions about how to behave in concordance with
their values and consciousness. A weakness of this
theory is, however, the difficulty to verify the ‘shi ’,
experimentally reveal the processes involved in
the shi s. Another weak point of this theory is the
possibility of misuse of it for the excuse of all bad
deeds committed on orders of authorities.
Milgram’s theory was developed for other
purposes (see note), but it can be successfully
applied to any explanation of absurd obedience
to authority, when orders are inconsistent with
the ethical standards of his subordinates (e.g. for
explanation of anti-environmental behaviour in
some companies).
The consequences of conflict between our
attitudes, but also between our thoughts and actions
are expressed in the theory of cognitive dissonance
(Festinger, 1957). If we are in the state of cognitive
dissonance, we experience unpleasant feelings of
tension, which force us to take the necessary steps to
reduce this tension. If a man has in his value system
internalized ecological behaviour, and for some
reason he made actions against these values and
beliefs (for example, secretly exported waste into
wood), usually he has three options how to deal with
cognitive dissonance which strikingly reminiscent
of bad conscience (Dundelová, 2011):
He/she can take away this (and possibly other)
waste from the forest; although it sounds simple
in reality it is the unlikeliest way of coping with
cognitive dissonance, because in this case people
have to denounce their previous bad behaviour and
thus suffers their self-esteem, thus we meet with this
response, I dare to say, really very rarely.
The second eventuality of solving this problem is
the trivialization of the problem when this polluter
excuses himself that he did not harm the forest
so much, that worse things happen in the world
and he basically had no choice (he was forced by
circumstances, he did not have enough money, etc.).
A third option is an addition of a new positive
value to this action; in this case for example “our
offender” can start to think that although he
established a new forbidden dump in the forest, he
spared money for the family (because he did not
have to pay a fee for a legal dump site) and that’s
a ‘good thing’.10
Cognitive dissonance can also be used for
enlarging the scale of pro-environmental behaviour;
in the literature is o en mentioned the technique
“foot-in-the-door-technique”. This technique is
based on the idea that if we are willing to cooperate
on a small project, then it is probable that we will
cooperate on larger events solving similar issues.
Results of many surveys (in Winter, Koger, 2009)
verify that people are more likely to fulfil their
promises to participate in the project (in our case
in a project with environmental objective), if their
names are public if they are “forced” to talk about
the project with neighbours or if they confirm their
participation with their signature.11
The difficulty to persuade individuals or groups
to cooperate is shown in a classic (but in Czech
literature less known) social psychological study
conducted by Sherif (1956) that demonstrated that
prejudices and animosity towards members of other
groups occurs when they are in competition for
the same resources. This was very important on the
heels of the Holocaust to explain how the Germans
(who saw the Jews as competition for economic
resources) could mutually support their egregious
acts against Jews.
In this study Sherif (1956) conducted a quite
complicated field experiment to examine group
dynamics. This experiment is known as the
Summer Camp; boys of age from 11 to 12 were
brought to a summer camp and they were randomly
assigned to one of two groups. During the first phase
of the experiment group members participated in
challenging tasks with each other (e.g. hiking); this
lead to creation of social hierarchies and also leaders
emerged.
9 for acts of genocide offered by those accused at the World War II, Nuremberg War Criminal trials. Their defense o en
was based on “obedience” – that they were just following orders of their superiors. The experiments began in July
1961, a year a er the trial of Adolf Eichmann in Jerusalem. Milgram devised the experiment to answer the question
“Could it be that Eichmann and his million accomplices in the Holocaust were just following orders? Could we call
them all accomplices?” (Milgram, 1974).
10 The first example represents the action, the redress of previous bad behaviour and also atonement. The second and
third solutions are typical examples of rationalization. /author’s note/
11 In marketing we meet quite a misuse of this technique. /author’s note/
2090
Jana Dundelová
During the second phase of the experiment, the
groups had to compete against each other in various
types of contests with trophies and prizes being
offered to the winning group. During this phase,
group cohesiveness increased while intergroup
conflict and animosity strengthened.
During the third phase of the experiment, Sherif
attempted to reconcile the two groups. This was
accomplished by presenting them with problems
in which they had to all work together to solve
them (and which successful solution was good for
all of them). They had to repair interrupted pipes
leading water from a tank or push a truck having
a “breakdown”. These activities were important
for all of them and required members from both
groups. These joint efforts did not immediately
dispel hostility but gradually frictions and conflicts
were reduced and new friendships were developed.
This finding can lead us to suggestions how to make
people cooperate and considerations if it is actually
feasible in the global extent.
4 POSSIBLE GLOBAL SOLUTIONS
The Sherif’s experiment shows how difficult it
is to make two groups that have developed mutual
rivalries and hatred to cooperate. The results of
this study also set a solution (probably the only
possible in this situation) – two or more groups
would have to face a common threat, in which all
of them are involved. In the case of environmental
issues, it is easy to imagine how extensive
damage or danger it should be … and moreover
the planet Earth is not small Sherif’s Summer
camp; organization, communication, dynamics,
hierarchies, relationships – this everything would
be modified in the global extend compared to the
model situation. Nevertheless, some authors try to
develop ecological concepts embracing the whole
planet, e.g. Campbell (1988), who introduced the
concept of “society of the planet”, which should
be interconnected society on the Earth that works in
harmony with the nature.
Shrivastava (in Stead and Stead, 1998) proposes
to implement into business organizations so called
“ecocentric management paradigm” that puts
nature into the center of interest of management
and organization. The problem is that managers are
usually trained only for one subsystem of the Earth
– the economic subsystem – which cannot exist
separately from the other subsystems.
Recently, we can encounter the concept
“environmental management”, which deals
with the issue of environmental problems, and
especially with their solution; the aim should be
to achieve the very o en discussed sustainable
development. Even though this issue is commonly
viewed from the planetary perspective, the key to
the solution must be in an attitudinal change of
the companies, or rather of the individuals who
work in them. An example can be the difference in
thinking about long-term planning in terms of age
of the Earth instead of in terms of human life, which
offers a completely different paradigm for decision
making. Unfortunately, for contemporary western
world the short-term thinking – the concentration
on the consumer way of life – is very typical in
which consumption prevails over life, and a genuine
interest in solving environmental problems is o en
mistaken for a mere effort to gain visibility, to attract
the public attention with the aim of getting public
support; and the ecological issues are abused for
advertising, marketing gimmicks with the only aim
to acquire the greatest number of customers.
The abovementioned concepts (society of the
planet, environmental management, ecocentric
management paradigm) seem to be idealistic,
even unrealistic and utopian, but their benefits
can be seen in visualization of environmental and
ecological issues, in establishing basements for
changes in only profit-oriented, and anti-ecological
and pseudoecological12 paradigms.
CONCLUSION
Probably it is not in human powers to prevent
major natural or evolutionary changes, but to treat
the nature according to our own conscience and
resist various pressures – psychological, economic
or political – it should be a challenge for each
responsible human. Even if we agreed with eco-
pessimists we had to cope with the question of
ethical values and respect for life, for the nature. The
boundary between ecology and pseudo-ecology will
always remain very thin and the results of scientific
researches as well as theories are o en contradictory
and we can guess that in some cases also apparently
purpose-built.
At the beginning of this paper some influential
and conflicting theories related to ecology were
discussed: according to some authors (e.g. Ehrlich,
1968, Gore, 2006, Wilson, 1995, Winter, Koger, 2009,
Šmajs 2005) human survival is directly connected
with people’s relationship to the nature; but other
influential authors have divergent opinions or they
are at least afraid of overestimation of ecological
activities that can lead to neglecting of other
important problems (e.g. Simon 1981, Goklany,
2007, Lomborg, 2007, Klaus, 2007, 2009).
As it was documented in the overview of selected
psychological theories and researches, all of them
described a human psychic phenomenon that
can be generally called destructive element (in
this context were mentioned Freud’s concept of
12 Pseudo-ecological paradigms (or concepts, products etc.) are presented as ecological, but their goal is not the genuine
interest in the environment and in solutions of ecological problems, but the visibility with the only goal of gain of their
promoters. /author’s note/
Issues of environmental (ir)responsibility
2091
unconscious – especially his idea of thanatos;
illusion of invulnerability, contingency trap or
social trap that corresponds with laziness, egoism;
conformity, obedience that corresponds to fear,
self-interest, egoism, fear to disagree with authority
or group, animosity, tragedy of commons that can
be related to individualism and short-sightedness
and egoism) which might positively correlate with
development and with the survival of the individual,
a group, but the question remains whether this
element cannot cause the extinction of entire
species, i.e. of humankind. We can seek footholds
in philosophy, education, and particularly in the
personal eco-responsibility, in general values, and in
questionable term “human’s conscience”. Of course
it is possible to argue that everyone has different
boundaries of their own conscience but those who
feel superior to the nature, should remember Albert
Einstein’s quote: “If the bee disappeared off the
surface of the earth, man would only have four years
le to live.”
SUMMARY
This paper is focused on the critical evaluation of humans’ relation to their environment, to the
nature. With the development of modern technologies is this issue increasingly relevant as well as
the question whether humans are able to affect fundamentally the environment on the Earth by their
activities.
The author reflects these issues mainly from the psychological point of view but also some influential
and conflicting theories related to ecology are discussed: according to some authors (e.g. Ehrlich, 1968,
Gore, 2006, Wilson, 1995, Winter, Koger, 2009, Šmajs 2005) human survival is directly connected with
people’s relationship to the nature; but other influential authors have divergent opinions or they are
at least afraid of overestimation of ecological activities that can lead to neglecting of other important
problems (e.g. Simon, 1981; Goklany, 2007; Lomborg, 2007; Klaus, 2007, 2009).
The core of this paper is devoted to psychological aspects that cause that some people have tendency
to environmentally protective behaviour while the others feel to be separated from nature or even to
be superior to it (in this context is mentioned Freud’s concept of eros versus thanatos, groupthink,
the illusion of invulnerability and rationalization, phenomena like obedience and conformity, the
concept “the Tragedy of the Commons”, the theory of cognitive dissonance, group dynamics and
Sherif’s experiments). This article is set into the political and economical frame and behind the lines
is emphasized that for understanding human behaviour to the nature is required more than the
knowledge of separated disciplines.
For contemporary western world is typical the short-term thinking and pseudo-ecological paradigms
(or concepts, products etc.) that are presented as ecological, but their goal is not the genuine interest
in the environment and in solutions of ecological problems, but the visibility with the only goal of
gain of their promoters. Finally, there are discussed possible solutions of ecological problems and
common obstacles that honest ecologists and environmentalists have to cope with.
REFERENCES
DUNDELOVÁ, J., 2011: Psychologické faktory
a procesy ovlivňující ekologické chování. In:
Enterprise and Competitive Environment 2011. 1. vyd.
Bučovice: Martin Kříž Publishing, s. 172–178.
ISBN 978-80-87106-40-2.
EHRLICH, P. R., 1968: The Population Bomb. New York:
Ballantine Books, 201 p. ISBN 1-56849-587-0.
FESTINGER, L., 1957: A Theory of Cognitive dissonance.
New impression edition Stanford: Standford
University Press, 291 p. ISBN 978-0804701310.
GOKLANY, I. M., 2007: The Improving State of the
World: Why We’re Living Longer, Healthier, More
Comfortable Lives on a Cleaner Planet. Washington:
Cato Institute, 450 p. ISBN-10 1930865988, ISBN-
13 978-1930865983.
GORE, Al., 2006: An Inconvenient Truth: The Planetary
Emergency of Global Warming and What We Can
Do About It. Emmaus, Pennsylvania: Rodale Press,
328 p. ISBN 1-59486-567-1.
HAYESOVÁ, N., 2007: Základy sociální psychologie.
Praha: Portál, 166 s. ISBN 978-80-7367-283-6.
HEWSTONE, M., STROEBE, W., 2006: Sociální
psychologie: moderní učebnice sociální psychologie. 1. vyd.
Praha: Portál, 769 s. ISBN 80-7367-092-5.
JURČÍK, R., 2007: The economic impact of the
EC procurement policy. Agricultural Economics-
Zemědelská ekonomika, 53, 7: 333–337. ISSN 0139-
570X.
KLAUS, V., 2007: Modrá, nikoli zelená planeta. Co je
ohroženo: klima, nebo svoboda?, Praha: Dokořán, 160 s.
ISBN 978-80-7363-152-9.
KLAUS, V., 2009: Modrá planeta v ohrožení (Sborník
nových textů o globálním oteplování) Praha: Dokořán,
228 s. ISBN 978-80-7363-277-9.
LOMBORG, B., 2007: Skeptický ekolog (Jaký je skutečný
stav světa?). Praha: Dokořán, 358s. ISBN 80-7363-
059-1.
MILGRAM, S., 1963: Behavioral Study of Obedience,
Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 67, 4: 371–
378. ISSN 0096-851X, doi:10.1037/h0040525.
2092
Jana Dundelová
MILGRAM, S., 1974: Obedience to Authority; An
Experimental View. New York: Harpercollins, 224 p.
ISBN 0-06-131983-X.
SHERIF, M., 1956: Experiments in group conflict.
Scientific American, 195, 54–58 p. ISSN 0272-4316.
STEAD, W. E., STEAD, J. G., 1998: Management pro
malou planetu: strategické rozhodování a životníprostředí.
1. vyd. Praha: G plus G, 284 s. ISBN 80-86103-15-3.
ŠMAJS, J., 2005: Základy systemické filosofie. Brno:
Masarykova univerzita v Brně, 255 s. ISBN 80-210-
3871-3.
VÝROST, J., 1998: Aplikovaná sociální psychologie I. 1.
vyd. Praha: Portál, 383 s. ISBN 80-7178-269-6.
WILSON, E. O., 1995: Rozmanitost života: Umožní
poznání zákonů biodiverzity její záchranu? Praha:
Lidové noviny, 444 s. ISBN 8071061131,
9788071061137.
WILSON, E. O., 1984: Biophilia. Cambridge: Harvard
University Press, 352 p. ISBN 0-674-07442-4.
WINTER, D. D. N., KOGER, S. M., 2009: Psychologie
environmentálních problémů. 1. vyd. Praha: Portál,
295 s. ISBN 978-80-7367-593-6.
Internet sources
HARDIN, G., 1968: Tragedy of the Commons
(1968) Science, 162, p. 1243–1248. [cit. 2012-
10-12]. Available from: http://www.cs.wright.
edu/~swang/cs409/Hardin.pdf.
The end of the Oil Age (Oct 23rd 2003) The
Economist [cit. 2012-12-12]. Available from: http://
www.economist.com/node/2155717.
Address
PhDr. Jana Dundelová, Ph.D., Department of Law and Social Sciences, Faculty of Business and Economics,
Mendel University in Brno, Zemědělská 1, 613 00 Brno, Czech Republic, e-mail: jana.dundelova@mendelu.
cz