Notions of animism in the West have existed
since the 6th century BCE (Harding 2013),
though Edward Tylor’s work in the late 1800s is
a commonly accepted origin (Bird-David, 1999;
Malville, 2016; Stringer, 2013). Tylor borrowed
the idea from Stahl, a 17th century alchemist
(Bird-David, 1999); however, contemporary
anthropologists and scholars of traditional
peoples deemed it morally unacceptable (Bird-
David, 1999; Malville, 2016; Stringer, 2013). Many
consider Tylor a father of cultural anthropology
of the “modernist” period, in which science and
evolutionism were held in the highest regard.
Through second-hand accounts of “primitive
peoples,” he defined a concept of animism. The
traditional/indigenous peoples that lived under
this banner believed in the existence of spirits,
whom were embodied in all human and non-
human entities. From this Tylor posited that
the minds of animistic traditional peoples were
similar to that of children who attribute living
qualities to inanimate objects, concluding that
their societies were cognitively underdeveloped
(Bird-David, 1999). This line of thought directly
stems from the evolutionistic mind frame of
the modernist period which presented animism
as the “root” of religion, and thus less evolved
in comparison to monotheistic religions
(Christianity, Islam, etc.), and importantly,
science. Tylor entered this field of study with
an interest in the spiritualist movement of
the time, which he argued was a “survival and
revival of savage thought” (Bird-David, 1999,
S69). His work on animism was collated and
presented in the 1871 book Primitive Culture:
Researches into the Development of Mytholo y,
Philosophy, Reli ion, Lan ua e, Art and Custom.
Whilst Tylor touched on important concepts
within cultural anthropology, his approach
was condescending to traditional peoples and
the subject retains a stigma to the present day
(Malville, 2016).
Reignited by Hallowell’s 1960’s ethnography of
the Ojibwa people and the intrinsic animation
of objects within their language, notable
anthropologists such as Eduardo Viveiros
de Castro, Philippe Descola, and Nurit Bird-
David began to revive the concept of animism
through the turn of the century and have
opened an inquiry into animism’s ideas about a
world alive (Harvey, 2013). In contrast to Tylor’s
positivist approach to notions of “life,” “nature,”
and “personhood,” contemporary scholars
have suggested a relational epistemolo y to
develop better understandings of “local
concepts” (Bird-David, 1999). Through what is
now viewed as misdirected understandings,
theoreticians in the modernist period assumed
“primitive peoples” shared the same notions
of “self” to natural objects, “primitive peoples”
were deemed misguided (Bird-David, 1999).
In contrast, relational epistemology seeks
to understand the world via a primary focus
on relatednesses (relationships between
the human and non-human) and avoids the
modernist dichotomies of natural/supernatural
and spirit/body (Bird-David, 1999).
New Animism: Relational Epistemology
“Animism is about a world full of immediate
relational beings” (Naveh & Bird-David, 2013,
27). Naveh & Bird-David’s (2013) chapter in
Graham Harvey’s Handbook of Contemporary
Animism explores the ideas of animism,
conservation, and immediacy (regular exposure
to something as in one’s everyday life). They
state that animism is absent from the West
due to the lack of immediacy ) in engagements
with plants, animals, and other natural objects.
They explore immediacy through ethnographic
observation of forest dwelling Nayaka people
in the Nilgiris hills in South India. The
region has become a hotspot for regional and
global ecological development and has been
recognised as a UNESCO biosphere reserve since
the 1970s. The authors state that during a 30-
year gap between ethnographic observations,
the Nayaka people have managed to retain
their relational epistemology despite economic
pressures. The Nayaka’s animistic epistemology
places greater significance on knowing how to
behave with relations to nourish them, over the
dualistic notion that things are separate from
oneself. Despite achieving conservation in the
region, it has not been cognitively pursued and
behaviour with relations has been shown to be
variable within the Nayaka. According to the
authors, tribal members display mindfulness
and care when harvesting or hunting for their
immediate use or consumption but not when
conducting similar activities for economic
means. Despite these con icting actions,
animism and relational epistemology present
opportunities for recognising contemporary
Western society’s utilitarian epistemologies
57