Nature as a Commodity: What’s Good for Human Health Might Not Be Good for Ecosystem Health
OPINION
published: 10 September 2018
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01673
Nature as a Commodity: What’s Good
for Human Health Might Not Be Good
for Ecosystem Health
Yolanda van Heezik 1* and Eric Brymer 2
1 Zoology Department, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand, 2 Institute of Sport, Physical Activity and Leisure, Leeds
Beckett University, Leeds, United Kingdom
Keywords: nature-related wellbeing, urban biodiversity, nature therapy, nature doses, greenspaces, nature
exposure
Edited by:
Patrik Sörqvist,
Gävle University College, Sweden
Reviewed by:
Sarah Bekessy,
RMIT University, Australia
*Correspondence:
Yolanda van Heezik
yolanda.vanheezik@otago.ac.nz
Specialty section:
This article was submitted to
Environmental Psychology,
a section of the journal
Frontiers in Psychology
Received: 28 March 2018
Accepted: 20 August 2018
Published: 10 September 2018
Citation:
van Heezik Y and Brymer E (2018)
Nature as a Commodity: What’s Good
for Human Health Might Not Be Good
for Ecosystem Health.
Front. Psychol. 9:1673.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01673
Are you getting enough Vitamin N? Richard Louv (2008) coined this term in his book “Last
Child in the Woods,” in response to growing evidence that suggests humans are increasingly
disinterested with, and disconnected from the natural world. Concurrent with the literature on
the extent of disconnection (Miller, 2005; Soga et al., 2016; van Heezik and Hight, 2017) is an
ever-expanding body of literature documenting the many psychological, physical, and spiritual
health benefits derived from nature contact (Keniger et al., 2013; Bratman et al., 2015; Martyn
and Brymer, 2016; Frumkin et al., 2017). In fact human survival is inextricably linked with nature:
the species and their inter-relationships that make up the fabric of ecosystems function to sustain
all life on Earth. Biodiversity in all landscapes, including urban ones, provides humans with
essential ecosystem services, such as food provisioning, climate and flood regulation, nutrient
cycling, carbon sequestration, and pollution reduction (Elmqvist et al., 2015). Frameworks have
been proposed for evaluating the economic value of biodiversity (Edwards and Abivardi, 1998),
with more recent approaches acknowledging the inter-play between social well-being, economic
sustainability, and biodiversity and ecosystem function (Tzoulas et al., 2007; Laurila-Pant et al.,
2015). These socio-cultural valuation techniques recognize that biodiversity provides society with
benefits, such as mental well-being, ethical, spiritual and cultural values, as well as economic
values. Psychological well-being benefits have been positively associated with the number of species
perceived by people in the environments around them (Fuller et al., 2007; Dallimer et al., 2012).
Loss of biodiversity reduces the efficiency by which ecological communities perform ecosystem
services, as well as the stability of ecosystem function over time (Cardinale et al., 2012).
The role that urban nature can play in enhancing psychological and physical well-being and
reducing health-related costs could be seen by those advocating for the protection and restoration
of urban biodiversity and ecosystem function as a positive outcome, suggesting a need to place
greater value on biodiverse urban spaces. Another, less positive, scenario is that the connection
between human health and nature might threaten the ecological integrity of urban green spaces
by commodifying nature, especially if green spaces are designed and managed for human health
benefits alone, with little concern for supporting biodiversity or ecosystem services. In this latter
scenario nature could become a “pill” with only those aspects of nature that most strongly
influence human health and wellbeing considered to be important in the design process. Here we
demonstrate how this undesirable outcome might be realized, and argue that a focus on treating
urban nature purely as an efficient means of delivering minimal levels of psychological well-being
is short-sighted. The development of knowledge and implementation of best practice that ensures
outcomes that provide for psychological well-being requires an interdisciplinary approach that
encourages diverse ecological communities with greater input by ecologists.
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org
1
September 2018 | Volume 9 | Article 1673
van Heezik and Brymer
Human Well-Being and Ecosystem Health
NATURE AS A THERAPEUTIC DEVICE
Mounting support for the link between contact with nature
and improved human health and well-being has led to nature
being applied for therapeutic purposes; for example, Shinrin-
yoku or “forest bathing” in Japan (Song et al., 2016; Hansen
et al., 2017), horticultural therapy or gardening (e.g., Clatworthy
et al., 2013; Kamioka et al., 2014), participation in woodland
management (Townsend, 2006), and green prescriptions (Van
den Berg, 2017). A systematic review of studies of nature-assisted
therapy revealed robust support for its effectiveness (Annerstedt
and Währborg, 2011). Evidence suggests that time in nature is
particularly beneficial for psychological health (Brymer et al.,
2014; Bragg and Atkins, 2016). Consequently, some researchers
have been focusing on identifying the minimum “doses” of nature
needed to benefit well-being (Shanahan et al., 2015; Cox et al.,
2017).
MINIMUM DOSES FOR WELL-BEING
BENEFITS; WHAT MIGHT THEY MEAN FOR
BIODIVERSITY?
How much nature exposure is required to derive a psychological
health benefit? Shanahan et al. (2016) focused on time spent
by people in nature and applied a dose-response analysis, used
in health contexts to evidence effectiveness: they found that
visits of 30+ min to green spaces could reduce the population
prevalence of depression and high blood pressure by 7 and 9%,
respectively, translating to savings for public health budgets.
Such evidence has influenced health-related decision-making
globally. In general, dose-response calculations have influenced
physical activity research and manifest as green prescriptions by
doctors, whereby people are encouraged to be more active, and
green spaces are promoted as beneficial. From this perspective,
psychological health benefits come about directly from the
fact that green spaces encourage physical activity. However
research also indicates green spaces have direct positive effects
on psychological health and well-being (Pretty et al., 2006;
Barton et al., 2016). Green prescriptions can therefore be
an important contribution to public health, and strategies to
encourage adoption of green prescriptions have been proposed
(Van den Berg, 2017).
While green prescriptions and recommendations on the
frequency and duration of exposure to nature might seem
helpful, or at least benign, Stanley et al. (2015) argue that
considering nature in this way has detrimental consequences
for biodiversity. Specifically, the growing numbers of people
accessing green spaces only for health benefits, together with the
promotion of health-related (including exercise) requirements
within green space design, threatens biodiversity and the
integrity of urban ecosystems. This is because green spaces
are inevitably modified to accommodate human use. Examples
include, pathways extended and widened, large flat areas (e.g.,
lawns) created for exercise groups, vegetation modified to
enhance users’ perceptions of safety, and artificial lighting
installed for use outside daylight hours (Stanley et al., 2015).
Habitat design, if undertaken purely from a health and well-being
perspective, might exclude species perceived as undesirable, such
as snakes or spiders. Often these green spaces are rated on
aesthetic characteristics and because aesthetic preferences do not
always align with habitat supporting biodiversity, recreational
spaces might provide resources for only the most tolerant
urban exploiters, which are often non-native (McKinney, 2002).
Less tolerant species are likely to abandon popular, well-lit
areas when frequent noise interferes with auditory cues, when
sounds are perceived as threats, and when pedestrians and
dogs interrupt foraging, resulting in more time being vigilant,
energy wasted, and foraging opportunities lost. While urban
green spaces might provide habitat for some hardy non-
human residents, paradoxically “people-friendly” spaces are
not necessarily “wildlife-friendly.” Thus design of green spaces
might need to consider a broader perspective than aesthetic
characteristics or the maximization of recreation activities.
Others have applied dose-response curves to estimate
the minimum levels of vegetation required for improved
well-being. Cox et al. (2017) evaluated five neighborhood
nature characteristics and calculated dose-responses for mental
disorders, concluding that quantifiable reductions in the
prevalence of poor mental health could be achieved with even
low levels of components of neighborhood nature. Another study
investigated the dose of nature required to reduce stress in people
subjected to a Trier Social Stress Test. Study participants watched
assigned street scenes with different tree densities; the male dose-
response curve indicated that stress reduction was greatest at tree
densities of 24–34% (Jiang et al., 2014).
While these studies provide valuable insights into the amount
and type of nature exposure necessary to effect improved
human well-being, this approach becomes problematic when
the minimal levels and type of vegetation identified as safe and
adequate to enhance human well-being are insufficient to support
biodiverse communities and stable ecosystem function. Tall trees
and shrub understoreys provide habitat for small mammals
(Dickman and Doncaster, 1987), birds (Jokimäki and Suhonen,
1993; van Heezik et al., 2008), and invertebrates (Smith et al.,
2006), and are an important generator of ecosystem services
(Gaston et al., 2013). Despite the important role that vegetation
volume plays in supporting biodiversity and ecosystem services,
trade-offs, and conflicts exist between planning for biodiversity
and planning for local residents. For example, these same rich
biodiverse habitats might also present health and safety issues
(e.g., dark parks, health problems from pollen, places for drug
taking activities). It is therefore feasible that those responsible for
greening urban environments might introduce vegetation based
on an easy-to-manage approach, rather than an approach that
considers local biodiversity and ecosystem services.
WHAT KIND OF NATURE?
Keniger et al. (2013) emphasized the importance of
understanding the characteristics of natural settings that
trigger well-being benefits and how these vary among cultural
and socioeconomic groups. However the kind of nature
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org
2
September 2018 | Volume 9 | Article 1673
van Heezik and Brymer
Human Well-Being and Ecosystem Health
researchers have focused on to demonstrate links to positive
well-being responses is frequently not reflective of the type of
natural environments conservationists seek to encourage. In
many studies on psychological well-being benefits the natural
environment is described as parkland with scattered shrubs and
trees (Bowler et al., 2010; Bratman et al., 2015). Descriptions of
“nature treatments” can be very broad. From a health perspective
the notions of greenness and nature often stem from what
the environment looks like, and biodiversity is either assumed
because the environment looks green or not considered at
all (Keniger et al., 2013; Shwartz et al., 2014; Sandifer et al.,
2015). This is because few studies specifically focusing on health
have involved ecologists. “Greenness” is measured remotely if
the focus of the study is on entire neighborhoods (e.g., Beyer
et al., 2014; Dadvand et al., 2016) or using generalized land-use
databases (e.g., Alcock et al., 2014; Gidlow et al., 2016). These
are convenient to use in population-level studies, but do not
represent many of the relevant features of the greenness, such
as species diversity and composition, vertical structure, and
wildness. In a review of 125 journal articles about green space,
fewer than half defined what the green spaces consisted of with
only simple generic descriptions provided; e.g., park, golf course
(Taylor and Hochuli, 2017). Moreover, when greenspace quality
was referred to, “quality” was subjectively determined without
reference to ecological integrity.
DOES USING NATURE TO ENHANCE
HUMAN WELL-BEING COMPROMISE
BIODIVERSITY?
Urban nature is increasingly seen as a manageable resource to
enhance human well-being. By viewing nature as a commodity
that supplies health benefits, and by identifying minimum
amounts needed to gain benefits, we risk trivializing a deep
affective response to nature. We might end up with a watered-
down, biodiversity-poor version of nature with compromised
ecosystem services. By creating a new baseline of what is
considered normal we could exacerbate ongoing shifts toward
more depleted environments. The concept of shifting baselines
(Pauly, 1995) is pertinent to each generation of urban residents
that perceive the state of the environments they encounter in
their childhood as normal, unaware of the past losses and the
depleted and altered nature of the biodiversity that remains.
Paradoxically, this could also reduce the psychological benefits
from human-nature interactions.
Research linking psychological wellbeing and nature has
traditionally focused on the individual psychological workings
of the individual or the form and structure of nature (e.g.,
color, objects and spaces between objects), often arguing for
“greenness” as the mediator for wellbeing (Brymer et al., 2014).
If well-being benefits, albeit minimal, can be gained from highly
modified, simplistic greenspaces, and these types of green spaces
become the new norm for the next generation, then there will be
little incentive to restore greenspaces to a more natural biodiverse
state, or even to protect what we currently have from degradation.
While it is still early days, in recent years research into
human health is acknowledging that the focus on “greenness”
is too simplistic and, when considering psychological well-
being, the “richness” of the environment and the human-nature
relationship is turning out to be of paramount importance
(Brymer et al., 2014; Fabjanski and Brymer, 2017; Lawton
et al., 2017). Therefore, an interdisciplinary approach, including
input from ecologists and health professionals, is essential
to optimize green space design for psychological well-being,
which will also ensure ecosystem well-being. A consensus on
greenspace definitions is necessary to provide a context for
such research (Taylor and Hochuli, 2017). Tools such as the
“Bioscore” developed by Hand et al. (2016), which incorporates
perceived diversity and human perceptions of naturalness, might
be applied to a variety of greenspaces (e.g., Müller et al.,
2018). Viewing nature as a “pill,” separate from humanity but
applied as required, is short-sighted. More meaningful gains for
human well-being can be achieved through recognition that the
artificial divide between people and nature is false. Developing
a culture of stewardship rather than one of exploitation, and
lifting biodiversity baselines through ecological restoration is
necessary. From a psychological health perspective, what is
urgently needed is a principled theoretical framework, combining
ecological, and psychological related knowledge that can guide
a more enlightened program of research and practice. Only
through this interdisciplinary approach, and the development
of frameworks that support this approach, will we promote
and protect the health and well-being of people and of
nature.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
YvH conceptualized the opinion piece and wrote a first draft. EB
contributed to concept and refined the manuscript.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We thank Philip Seddon and a reviewer for constructive
comments.
REFERENCES
Alcock, I., White, M. P., Wheeler, B. W., Fleming, L. E., and Depledge,
M. H. (2014). Longitudinal effects on mental health of moving to
greener and less green urban areas. Environ. Sci. Technol. 48, 1247–1255.
doi: 10.1021/es403688w
Annerstedt, M., and Währborg, P. (2011). Nature-assisted therapy: systematic
review of controlled and observational studies. Scand. J. Public Health 39,
371–88. doi: 10.1177/1403494810396400
Barton, J., Bragg, R., Wood, C., and Pretty, J. (eds). (2016). Green
Exercise: Linking Nature, Health and Well-being. Abingdon: Earthscan/
Routledge.
Beyer, K. M. M., Kaltenbach, A., Szabo, A., Bogar, S., Nieto, F. J., and Malecki, K.
M., (2014). Exposure to neighborhood green space and mental health: evidence
from the survey of the health of Wisconsin. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health
11, 3453–3472. doi: 10.3390/ijerph110303453
Bowler, D. E., Buyung-Ali, L. M., Knight, T. M., and Pullin, A. S.
(2010). A systematic review of evidence for the added benefits to
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org
3
September 2018 | Volume 9 | Article 1673
van Heezik and Brymer
Human Well-Being and Ecosystem Health
health of exposure to natural environments. BMC Public Health 10:456.
doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-10-456
Bragg, R., and Atkins, G. (2016). A Review of Nature-Based Interventions for Mental
Health Care. Natural England Commissioned Reports.
Bratman, G. N., Hamilton, J. P., Hahn, K. S., Daily, G. C., and Gross,
J. J. (2015). Nature experience reduces rumination and subgenual
prefrontal cortex activation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 112, 8567–8572.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1510459112
Brymer, E., Davids, K., and Mallabon, E. (2014). Understanding the psychological
health and well-being benefits of physical activity in nature: an ecological
dynamics analysis. J. Ecopsychol. 6, 189–197. doi: 10.1089/eco.2013.0110
Cardinale, B. J., Duffy, J. E., Gonzalez, A., Hooper, D. U., Perrings, C., Venail, P.,
et al. (2012). Biodiversity loss and its impact on humanity. Nature 486, 59–67.
doi: 10.1038/nature11148
Clatworthy, J., Hinds, J., and Camic, P. M. (2013). Gardening as a
mental health intervention: a review. Ment. Health Rev. 18, 214–225.
doi: 10.1108/MHRJ-02-2013-0007
Cox, D. T. C., Shanahan, D. F., Hudson, H. L., Fuller, R. A., Anderson, K.,
Hancock, S., et al. (2017). Doses of nearby nature simultaneously associated
with multiple health benefits. Int. J. Env. Res. Public Health 14:E172.
doi: 10.3390/ijerph14020172
Dadvand, P., Bartoll, X., Basagaña, X., Dalmau-Bueno, A., Martinez, D., Ambros,
A., et al. (2016). Green spaces and general health: roles of mental health
status, social support and physical activity. Environ. Int. 91, 161–167.
doi: 10.1016/j.envint.2016.02.029
Dallimer, M., Irvine, K. N., Skinner, A. M. J., Davies, Z. G., Rouquette, J. R.,
Maltby, L. L., et al. (2012). Biodiversity and the feel-good factor: associations
between self-reported human well-being and species richness. Bioscience 62,
47–55. doi: 10.1525/bio.2012.62.1.9
Dickman, C. R., and Doncaster, C. P. (1987). The ecology of small mammals
in urban habitats. I. Populations in a patchy environment. J. Anim. Ecol. 56,
629–640. doi: 10.2307/5073
Edwards, P. J., and Abivardi, C. (1998). The value of biodiversity:
where ecology and economy blend. Biol. Conserv. 83, 239–246.
doi: 10.1016/S0006-3207(97)00141-9
Elmqvist, T., Setälä, H., Handel, S. N., van der Ploeg, S., Aronson, J., Blignaut, J.
N., et al. (2015). Benefits of restoring ecosystem services in urban areas. Curr.
Opin. Environ. Sustain. 14, 101–108. doi: 10.1016/j.cosust.2015.05.001
Fabjanski, M., and Brymer, E. (2017). Enhancing health and wellbeing through
immersion in nature: a conceptual perspective combining the Stoic and
Buddhist traditions. Front. Psychol. 8:1573. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01573
Frumkin, H., Bratman, G. N., Breslow, S. J., Cochran, B., Kahn Jr, P. H., Lawler, J.
J., et al. (2017). Nature contact and human health: a research agenda. Environ.
Health Persp. 125:075001. doi: 10.1289/EHP1663
Fuller, R. A., Irvine, K. N., Devine-Wright, P., Warren, P. H., and Gaston, K. J.
(2007). Psychological benefits of greenspace increase with biodiversity. Biol.
Lett. 3, 390–394. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2007.0149
Gaston, K. J., Ávila-Jiménez, M. L., and Edmonson, J. L. (2013). Managing
urban ecosystems for goods and services. J. Appl. Ecol. 50, 830–840.
doi: 10.1111/1365-2664.12087
Gidlow, C. J., Randall, J., Gillman, J., Smith, G. R., and Jones, M. V. (2016). Natural
environments and chronic stress measured by hair cortisol. Landscape Urban
Plan. 148, 61–67. doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2015.12.009
Hand, K., Freeman, C., Seddon, P. J., Stein, A., and van Heezik, Y. (2016).
A novel method for fine-scale biodiversity assessment and prediction
across diverse urban landscapes. Landscape Urban Plan. 151, 33–44.
doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2016.03.002
Hansen, M. M., Jones, R., and Tocchini, K., (2017). Shinrin-Yoku (Forest bathing)
and nature therapy: a state-of-the-art review. Int. J. Env. Res. Public Health
14:851. doi: 10.3390/ijerph14080851
Jiang, B., Chang, C.-Y., and Sullivan, W. C. (2014). A dose of nature: tree cover,
stress reduction, and gender differences. Landscape Urban Plan. 132, 26–36.
doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2014.08.005
Jokimäki, J., and Suhonen, J. (1993). Effects of urbanisation on the breeding bird
species richness in Finland: a biogeographical comparison. Ornis Fenn. 70,
71–77.
Kamioka, H., Tsutani, K., Yamada, M., Park, H., Okuizumi, H., Honda,
T., et al. (2014). Effectiveness of horticultural therapy: a systematic
review of randomized controlled trials. Compl. Ther. Med. 22, 930–943.
doi: 10.1016/j.ctim.2014.08.009
Keniger, L. E., Gaston, K. J., Irvine, K. N., and Fuller, R. A. (2013). What are
the benefits of interacting with nature? Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 10,
913–935. doi: 10.3390/ijerph10030913
Laurila-Pant, M.,Lehikoinen, A., Uusitalo, L., and Venesjärvi, R. (2015). How
to value biodiversity in environmental management? Ecol. Indic. 55, 1–11.
doi: 10.1016/j.ecolind.2015.02.034
Lawton, E., Brymer, E., Clough, P., and Denovan, A. (2017). The relationship
between the physical activity environment, nature relatedness, anxiety and the
psychological wellbeing benefits of regular exercisers. Front. Psychol. 8:1058.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01058
Louv, R. (2008). Last Child in the Woods: Saving Our Children from Nature Deficit
Disorder. Chapel Hill, NC: Algonquin Books.
Martyn, P., and Brymer, E. (2016). The relationship between nature relatedness
and anxiety. J. Health Psychol. 21, 1436–1445. doi: 10.1177/1359105314555169
McKinney, M. L. (2002). Urbanization, biodiversity, and conservation: the
impacts of urbanization on native species are poorly studied, but educating
a highly urbanized human population about these impacts can greatly
improve species conservation in all ecosystems. Bioscience 52, 883–890.
doi: 10.1641/0006-3568(2002)052[0883:UBAC]2.0.CO;2
Miller, J. R. (2005). Biodiversity conservation and the extinction of experience.
Trends Ecol. Evol. 20, 430–434. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2005.05.013
Müller, A., Bøcher, P. K., Fischer, C., and Svenning, J. C. (2018). Wild’ in the
city context: do relative wild areas offer opportunities for urban biodiversity?
Landscape Urban Plan. 170, 256–265. doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2017.
09.027
Pauly, D. (1995). Anecdotes and the shifting baseline syndrome of fisheries. Trends
Ecol. Evol. 10:430. doi: 10.1016/S0169-5347(00)89171-5
Pretty, J., Peacock, J., Sellens, M., and Griffin, M. (2006). The
mental and physical health outcomes of green exercise. Int. J.
Environ. Health Res. 15, 319–337. doi: 10.1080/096031205001
55963
Sandifer, P. A., Sutton-Grier, A. E., and Ward, B. P. (2015). Exploring connections
among nature, biodiversity, ecosystem services, and human health and well-
being: opportunities to enhance health and biodiversity conservation. Ecosyst.
Serv. 12, 1–15. doi: 10.1016/j.ecoser.2014.12.007
Shanahan, D. F., Bush, R., Gaston, K. J., Lin, B. B., Dean, J., Barber, E., et al. (2016).
Health benefits from nature experiences depend on dose. Sci. Rep. 6:28551.
doi: 10.1038/srep28551
Shanahan, D. F., Fuller, R. A., Bush, R., Lin, B. B., and Gaston, K. J. (2015). The
health benefits of urban nature: how much do we need? Bioscience 65, 476–485.
doi: 10.1093/biosci/biv032
Shwartz, A., Turb,é, A., Julliard, R., Simon, L., and Prévot, A.-C. (2014).
Outstanding challenges for urban conservation research and action.
Glob. Environ. Change 28, 39–49. doi: 10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2014.
06.002
Smith, R. M., K. J., Gaston, P. H., Warren, and Thompson, K. (2006). Urban
domestic gardens (VIII): environmental correlates of invertebrate abundance.
Biodiv. Conserv. 15, 2515–2545. doi: 10.1007/s10531-005-2784-y
Soga, M., Gaston, K. J., Koyanagi, T. F., Kurisu, K., and Hanaki, K.
(2016). Urban residents’ perceptions of neighbourhood nature: does
the extinction of experience matter? Biol. Conserv. 203, 143–150.
doi: 10.1016/j.biocon.2016.09.020
Song, C., Ikei, H., and Miyazaki, Y. (2016). Physiological effects of nature therapy:
a review of the research in Japan. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 13:E781.
doi: 10.3390/ijerph13080781
Stanley, M. C., Beggs, J. R., Bassett, I. E., Burns, B. R., Dirks, K. N., Jones, D. N., et al.
(2015). Emerging threats in urban ecosystems: a horizon scanning exercise.
Front. Ecol. Environ. 13, 553–560. doi: 10.1890/150229
Taylor, L., and Hochuli, D. F. (2017). Defining greenspace: multiple
uses across multiple disciplines. Landscape Urban Plan. 158, 25–38.
doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2016.09.024
Townsend, M. (2006). Feel Blue? Touch Green! Participation in forest/woodland
management as a treatment for depression. Urban For. Urban Gree. 5, 111–120.
doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2006.02.001
Tzoulas, K., Korpela, K., Venn, S., Yli-Pelkonen, V., Kazmierczak, A., Niemela,
J., et al. (2007). Promoting ecosystem and human health in urban areas using
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org
4
September 2018 | Volume 9 | Article 1673
van Heezik and Brymer
Human Well-Being and Ecosystem Health
Green Infrastructure: a literature review. Landscape Urban Plan. 81, 167–178.
doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2007.02.001
Van den Berg, A. E. (2017). From green space to green prescriptions:
challenges and opportunities for research and practice. Front. Psychol. 8:268.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00268
van Heezik, Y., and Hight, S. (2017). Socio-economic-driven differences in bird
feeding practices exacerbate existing inequities in opportunities to see native
birds in cities. J. Urban Ecol. 3:jux01. doi: 10.1093/jue/jux011
van Heezik, Y., Smyth, A., and Mathieu, R. (2008). Diversity of native and exotic
birds across and urban gradient in a New Zealand city. Landscape Urban Plan.
87, 223–232. doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2008.06.004
Conflict of Interest Statement: The authors declare that the research was
conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could
be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Copyright © 2018 van Heezik and Brymer. This is an open-access article distributed
under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The
use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the
original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original
publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice.
No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these
terms.
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org
5
September 2018 | Volume 9 | Article 1673