Sourcing Image Formation in a Depth Psychological Approach to Posttraumatic Stress Disorder
Sourcing Image Formation in a Depth Psychological Approach
to Posttraumatic Stress Disorder
by
Priscilla Newton
Submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements
for the degree of
Master of Arts in Counseling Psychology
Pacifica Graduate Institute
5 February 2015
UMI Number: 1690651
All rights reserved
INFORMATION TO ALL USERS
The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted.
In the unlikely event that the author did not send a complete manuscript
and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if material had to be removed,
a note will indicate the deletion.
UMI 1690651
Published by ProQuest LLC (2015). Copyright in the Dissertation held by the Author.
Microform Edition © ProQuest LLC.
All rights reserved. This work is protected against
unauthorized copying under Title 17, United States Code
ProQuest LLC.
789 East Eisenhower Parkway
P.O. Box 1346
Ann Arbor, MI 48106 - 1346
ii
© 2015 Priscilla Newton
All rights reserved
iii
I certify that I have read this paper and that in my opinion it conforms to acceptable
standards of scholarly presentation and is fully adequate, in scope and quality, as a
product for the degree of Master of Arts in Counseling Psychology.
____________________________________
Angela Mohan, M.A., L.M.F.T.
Portfolio Evaluator
On behalf of the thesis committee, I accept this paper as partial fulfillment of the
requirements for Master of Arts in Counseling Psychology.
____________________________________
Sukey Fontelieu, Ph.D., L.M.F.T.
Research Associate
On behalf of the Counseling Psychology program, I accept this paper as partial
fulfillment of the requirements for Master of Arts in Counseling Psychology.
____________________________________
Avrom Altman, M.A., L.M.F.T., L.P.C.
Director of Research
iv
Abstract
Sourcing Image Formation in a Depth Psychological Approach
to Posttraumatic Stress Disorder
by Priscilla Newton
Although recognition of the constellation of emotional and mental events that comprise
trauma date back to the late 19th century, contemporary curative approaches are derived
from World War II desensitization treatments for battle fatigue that appear today in
exposure-based therapies. To bring trauma therapy up to date, application of a
phenomenological research method to critique contemporary treatments for posttraumatic
stress disorder requires a multisystemic approach to the mindbody constellation that is
clinically informed by state-of-the-art neuroscientific research relating to the
pathophysiology of trauma. Particularly important is understanding the necessity of
cross-hemispheric integration of emotional and cognitive imprints that persist after
traumatic events. Mindfulness-based therapies such as the metacognitive model, emotion-
focused therapies, and breath training such as samatha meditation techniques as well as
the incorporation of somatic and ecopsychological approaches such as saltwater
immersion are all effective, long-term, compassionate solutions that create lasting
recovery from trauma and its adverse consequences to the life and well-being of the
trauma victim.
v
Acknowledgments
Many thanks to my family and friends who have supported me through the
writing process. Special thanks to Byron Debre, Sukey Fontelieu, Sonja Taneska-Proctor,
Jan Freya, Angela Mohan, and all of the members of my cohort at Pacifica Graduate
Institute.
vi
Dedication
This is dedicated to the wounded veterans and their sacrifices that have created the
incredible freedoms we enjoy as Americans.
vii
Table of Contents
Chapter I Introduction..................................................................................................1
Area of Interest ........................................................................................................1
Guiding Purpose.......................................................................................................2
Rationale ..................................................................................................................2
Methodology ............................................................................................................3
Research Problem ........................................................................................3
Research Question .......................................................................................4
Research Methodology ................................................................................4
Overview of the Thesis ............................................................................................5
Chapter II Literature Review.........................................................................................6
Image........................................................................................................................7
A Neuroscientific Theory on the Formation of Complexes.....................................9
The Neuroscience of Image and Trauma Formation .............................................10
Posttraumatic Stress Disorder ................................................................................13
PTSD‘s Co-Occurrence With Other Disorders..........................................15
The Neural Basis of PTSD and Processing Extreme Stress ......................17
Image Processing in PTSD ........................................................................20
Exposure-Based Interventions for PTSD...............................................................21
Eye Movement Desensitization and Reprocessing (EMDR).....................21
Virtual Reality Exposure Therapy (VRET) ...............................................21
Prolonged Exposure Therapy (PE) ............................................................23
Imagery Treatments ...............................................................................................25
Summary ................................................................................................................26
Chapter III Image, Neuroplasticity, and Alternative Therapies ...................................28
The Role of Image in the Healing Process.............................................................28
A Critique of Exposure Therapies .........................................................................29
Neuroscientific Contributions to the Pathophysiology of PTSD ...........................32
Neuroplasticity and Behavior Change in PTSD ....................................................33
Metacognitive Therapy: An Alternative to Prolonged Exposure ..........................35
Hemispheric Integration, Emotion-Focused Therapy, and
Mindfulness Practices ............................................................................................37
The Scientific Logic of Somatic Therapies ...........................................................41
Deep Saltwater Immersion Therapy (DSIT)..........................................................43
Chapter IV Summary and Conclusions ........................................................................47
Summary ................................................................................................................47
Conclusions ............................................................................................................48
viii
Implications of the Research for Psychotherapy .......................................49
Recommendations for Further Research....................................................49
Chapter I
Introduction
Area of Interest
It is epochal of the modern age that new technology will quickly outpace old
reforms and create new paradigms as well as greater complexity. Today, trauma
treatment is informed by an ever-growing technical and clinical map of its origins that
expands our understanding of its neurophysiology and insinuates progressive treatments
in the future. Recognition of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) as a syndrome dates as
far back as the late 19th century to the French neurologist Jean-Martin Charcot‘s
diagnosis of hysteria and the research conducted at the hallowed institution of Salpêtrière,
a psychiatric teaching hospital in Paris, in its day, ―a temple of modern science‖
(Herman, 1997, p. 10). In her documentation of the era‘s interest in hysteria, trauma
specialist Judith Herman (1997) reported that Charcot‘s Tuesday Lectures were
punctuated with the actual screams of hysterical patients to illustrate this mysterious
condition for the interest of students and medical luminaries alike. Pierre Janet, William
James, and Sigmund Freud all came to investigate and observe Charcot‘s work at
Salpêtrière (p. 10). Herman stated that, later, with the wars of the 20th century, hysteria
transmogrified into new diagnoses: shell shock, after World War I, and after World War
II, combat neurosis. In the wake of the Vietnam War and feminist research on rape
victims, which notably echoes Charcot‘s earlier work with hysterical patients (pp. 20-28),
PTSD was officially recognized by the American Psychiatric Association (1980) and
2
introduced in its third edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders. As found historically with psychological treatments that are due for
revisioning, I have noticed that there is sometimes more guesswork and posturing
regarding effective treatment for PTSD than there are actual results with longitudinal
efficacy. This discovery quickened my curiosity and innervated my wish to investigate.
Guiding Purpose
Trauma and its victims are part of the everyday agenda for the clinician who
works in the field. How then does the responsible depth psychologist effectively
approach the reality of trauma in a clinical setting while simultaneously maintaining
loyalty to the theoretical underpinnings of the depth tradition based in Jungian
psychology? The deeper one delves into the issues of trauma, the more apparent it
becomes that trauma is pervasive and, from the perspective of PTSD, that it is
understudied in terms of its co-occurrence with other major disorders. Furthermore, a
large research base promulgates an amalgam of treatments for PTSD, yet ultimate
questions remain regarding their efficacy. In light of the fact that, in this period of
history, technical advances frequently outpace our ability to know or even infer their
implications, as clinicians it is important that we continually reevaluate new research for
its applicability to our field and commit ourselves to using this knowledge to advance
treatment paradigms and improve client care.
Rationale
In some cases, when reviewing the actual techniques of certain persisting
treatments for PTSD, the only explanation for their continued use seems to be more a
commitment to the researchers‘ agendas and profiles in the scientific community than to
3
the actual well-being of the individuals they seek to treat. As healers, it is important that
we sacrifice hubris regarding our status in the profession in favor of humility that allows
others to contribute better ideas. This thesis aims to evaluate the field of trauma
psychology by leaning into the advances and advantages of neuroscientific technical
imaging and, in that process, query favored treatments, investigate actual levels of
treatment efficacy, and reevaluate their curative promise. Just as technical images inform
modern understanding of trauma psychology, so does image drive the psyche in trauma.
Analytical theorist James Hillman (1981) noted that ―images are the fundamentals which
make the movements of psychodynamics possible‖ (p. 7). PTSD can thus be seen
conceptually as a conglomeration of trauma-driven images that persistently drive
behavior through manifestations of its imprint in the psyche.
Methodology
Research problem. Image can serve as a foundation for our modern
understanding of PTSD and contribute both conceptually, clinically, and curatively to the
etiology and treatment of this pervasive condition. Current research findings revealing
damage to neuroanatomical structures caused by the phenomenon of trauma and the long-
term personal and social consequences related to the symptomology of PTSD demand a
reevaluation of older treatment paradigms as well as a forward-looking appraisal of
alternative and more efficacious approaches to a cure. Research has now shown that
PTSD is a multisystemic mind-body condition that affects a person emotionally,
cognitively, physiologically, neurophysiologically, and neuroanatomically (van der Kolk,
2001, pp. 49-51). Today, the treatment of choice for PTSD is exposure therapies. These
techniques have proven to fall short in bedside manner in the area of patient care as well
4
as longitudinally with questionable scores in long-term efficacy (Ghafoori & Davaie,
2012; Wells, Walton, Lovell, & Proctor, 2014). Because of the multisystemic nature of
PTSD, treatments need to envision compassionate avenues to emotional and cognitive
integration of traumatic memory as well as incorporate a holistic mind-body approach.
These alternatives can be found in metacognitive psychology, emotion-focused therapies,
mindfulness practices, as well as in somatic treatments. In addition, a depth psychological
approach founded in the traditional of analytical psychology can accommodate a
compassionate therapeutic alliance that reframes the experience of trauma in the psyche
and opens broad potentials for dialectic possibilities between therapist and client.
Research question. Through an introspective investigation of its role as a
common point in both the depth and clinical interpretations of PTSD, this thesis seeks to
investigate the ubiquitous presence of image and its influence on the mind. My research
question is the following: How can we, as depth psychologists, cross over from the depth
tradition into the larger clinical community and apply responsible and innovative depth
psychological treatments for trauma?
Research methodology. A phenomenological research method allows an
experiential overview of the qualitative research relating to the etiology of trauma, the
goals of treatment compared to their effect and efficacy, and proposed alternatives to
treatment in light of a contemporary state-of-the-art evidence base. Phenomenological
philosopher Maurice Merleau-Ponty (1964/1968) noted that,
[phenomenological reflection] must suspend the faith in the world only so as to
see it, only so as to read in it the route it has followed in becoming a world for us;
it must seek in the world itself the secret of our perceptual bond with it. . . . It
must question the world, it must enter into the forest of references that our
interrogation arouses in it, it must make it say, finally, what in its silence it means
to say. (pp. 38-39)
5
Phenomenology can reach beyond the subjective experience of illness and embrace the
real-world experience of trauma survivors (Finlay, 2012, p. 172). In process and practice,
this methodology opens the way to a cohesive theoretical map that fosters the objective
analysis of relevant issues and fosters a knowledgeable and engaged approach to the
study of trauma, its etiology, and treatment paradigms.
No apparent ethical conflicts exist within this approach to the research. The
evaluative critique of certain treatment paradigms in this thesis is intended to clarify
relevant issues relating to these modalities.
Overview of the Thesis
Chapter II defines trauma in terms of image through a depth psychological lens.
The etiology of trauma is further explicated in terms of complexes and archetypes and
then physiologically defined in an exact neuroscientific context. The chapter concludes
with an explanation of the implications of these studies‘ findings in the context of current
trauma treatment practices. In Chapter III, the role of image is extrapolated in the context
of recovery. PTSD treatments are reviewed through a neuroscientific lens that analyses
the potential of the change process offered by neuroplasticity and the neuroscientific
contributions to the pathophysiology of PTSD. The conclusion offers proposals for
specific therapeutic alternatives with a brief review of creative somatic options. Chapter
IV critiques the role of exposure therapies and reframes the viability of PTSD treatments
in light of current neuroscientific findings. An overview of alternative therapies is
reviewed.
Chapter II
Literature Review
Over the last 2 decades, technological developments in the area of neuroscientific
research have transformed the contemporary understanding of the neurophysiological and
psychological effects of trauma, its developmental trajectory, and the life-altering
consequences of untreated trauma. At the same time as neuroscientific research provides
access to long-held secrets of the brain, it also reveals the neuropathological
consequences of trauma and explains why the imprints of trauma continue to haunt its
victims unrelentingly. With the advent of advanced imaging technology, trauma research
has advanced the modern ability to map specific brain states, thus accelerating advances
in treatment that contribute to the current understanding of trauma and paradigms of
trauma resolution.
Normally, full resolution of an event occurs when perceptions are presented as
images in prelingual parts of the brain that are then processed and integrated as cognitive
events (van der Kolk, 2001, p. 56). In trauma, integration of certain images is stalled by
an individual‘s proximity to a powerful event that leads to a subsequent failure of
cognitive and cross-hemispheric processing (p. 56). Trauma then takes the form of a
conditioned sequence of images that dysregulates emotional processing and contributes to
related thinking and behavior that conforms to the trauma‘s negative imprint (pp. 56-57).
Today, exposure therapies are frequently the treatment of choice despite statistics
that point to their questionable efficacy (Wells et al., 2014; van Minnen, Harned,
7
Zoellner, & Mills, 2012). These modalities attempt to integrate trauma memories through
accessing prelingual and nonrational image processing centers in the brain in an effort to
desensitize imprinted trauma-based reactions and therefore reprogram future cognitive
and physiological responses to life events. Exposure treatments, however, are slowly
being outmoded because of advances in the knowledge base relating to trauma and the
possibilities offered by alternative treatments that focus more globally on healing trauma.
The research question provided in Chapter I queries the role of image processing
in trauma and searches for alternative answers to current treatment paradigms. Depth
psychology, and by extension analytical psychology, sheds extensive light on image and
the leading roles it plays in the human psyche. Interestingly, the implications of these
roles are also revealed from a scientific perspective by technical image processing and its
ability to expose brain activity through images. Recounted below is an analysis of trauma
from the perspectives of both analytical psychology and neuroscience followed by an
overview of contemporary treatment modalities. Crossing over in both traditions is the
preeminent and critical role of image that is inherent to the attribution and analysis of
image processing in singular approaches to trauma.
Image
Image is the foundation of the Jungian concept of the self and also of analytical
psychology‘s theory of archetypes. Jungian analyst Edward Whitmont (1991) agreed that
image rests in thinking and emerges vertically upwards from the depth of the psyche out
of the collective unconscious as a preverbal and visual notice of the psyche‘s
communication (p. 28). Eric Shalit (2010), also a Jungian analyst, viewed image as no
less than a ―nuclear force around which the images of soul and psyche gravitate‖ (p. 86).
8
Pioneering depth psychologist Carl G. Jung (1952/1980) moved this idea further into
theology and reflected that man is made in God‘s image:
God has indeed made an inconceivably sublime and mysteriously contradictory
image of himself, without the help of man, and implanted it in man‘s unconscious
as an archetype. . . . This image contains everything he will ever imagine
concerning his gods or concerning the ground of his psyche. (p. 667)
Referring to this archetype, depth psychologist and trauma expert Donald Kalsched
(1996) commented,
Among the many such coincidenta oppositora in the deep unconscious is one
central archetype which seems to stand for the very principle of unity among all
the opposing elements of the psyche and which participates in their volcanic
dynamism. This central organizing agency in the collective psyche is what Jung
called the archetype of the Self, both light and dark. It is characterized by
extraordinary luminosity, and an encounter with it can involve either salvation or
dismemberment, depending on which side of the Self‘s numinosity is
experienced by the ego. . . . Until the ego develops, the unified self cannot
actualizebut once constellated, it becomes the ground of the ego and its guide
in the rhythmic unfolding of the individual‘s inborn personality potential. (p. 18)
Hillman (1981) further explained,
When image is thus transposed from a human representation of its conditions to a
sui generis activity of soul in independent presentation of its bare nature, all
empirical studies on imagination, dream, fantasy, and the creative process in
artists . . . will contribute little to the psychology of the image if they start with the
empirics of imagining rather than with the phenomenon of the imagewhich is
not a product of imagining. (p. 8)
What are the ramifications of this primacy of image, and what is the influence of image
on mentation, behavior, and mental illness? According to Hillman (1981), image is the
unadulterated core of self perception that relates intimately to how the self processes
reality. By inference, then, image must also be implicated in the formation of mental
illness due to non-reality-based image processing.
9
A Neuroscientific Theory on the Formation of Complexes
From his theory of image formation, Jung extrapolated the emergence of
complexes that result during environmental interactions that eventually prevail over
reality-based perceptions (Silverstein, 2014, pp. 101-102) with negative developmental
consequences. Psychologist Steven Silverstein (2014), who assessed the validity of
Jung‘s ideas on schizophrenia in light of current neuroscience, related Jung‘s complex
theory to a combination of images and ideas held together by a strong feeling that
influences perception and behavior (p. 102). Jungian analyst Murray Stein (2008) made
the distinction that in contrast to the ego complex, which creates focus, complexes bind
motivation and definition in the psyche in a way that offers the possibility of unification
with the divine (p. 307). Drawing from Jung‘s theory, Silverstein (2014) explained that a
complex becomes an autonomous network of associations, separate in character and
nature from the individual, that continues to grow in strength as events transpire and
memories assimilate into the emergent complex, which then negatively interferes with
ego function. Eventually, he said, decision-making and conscious action is overseen by
the unconscious habituated pattern of a complex instead of spontaneous reality-based
interpretations generated by the ego complex.
From a neuroscientific point of view, the formation of a typical trauma complex is
consistent with mid-20th-century neuropsychologist Donald O. Hebb‘s notion that ―cells
that fire together wire together‖ (Silverstein, 2014, p. 104) and the premise of
neuroplasticity (Davidson, Jackson, & Kalin, 2000, p. 890). Consistent with observations
on the formation of neurological networks early in life (Davidson et al., 2000), Silverstein
(2014) posited that ―plasticity in the neural circuitry underlying emotion is likely to play
10
an important role in understanding the impact of early environmental factors in
influencing later individual differences and risk of psychopathology‖ (p. 890). According
to Silverstein, a complex can thus be considered to be formed by the mutual excitation of
neurons that develop through resonant mental or environmental events and bond in
networks that form or strengthen the network. He proposed that an exponential see-saw
effect occurs between the ego and the complex whereby, as the ego weakens, the
complex strengthens and distorts accurate perceptions of reality in favor of the tilt that is
the character of the complex (p. 107). He added, ―When cortical connectivity is
weakened in general, any newly formed network will be less influenced than it should be
by ongoing brain activity, and, by extension, by the statistical structure of reality‖
(p. 107). Silverstein proposed that complexes synergistically replace weaker ego-
congruent networks with ones characterized by the hyperexcitability of traumatic or
otherwise powerful events and thereafter construct reality through the dystonic neurosis
of the stronger interference (p. 107).
The Neuroscience of Image and Trauma Formation
Neuroscientific research has found that mental images are traced as brain activity
in the ventral and dorsal pathways. This event occurs during visual perception of an
actual object as well as during mental visualization in the absence of an object (Mazoyer,
Tzourio-Mazoyer, Mazard, Denis, & Mellet, 2002, p. 205). In other words, images based
on language, such as in the process of mentalizing directions, also have the same
psychophysical visual coding in the brain as images that derive from actual sight (p. 205).
Images therefore form in the mind based on verbal instructions and can also be derived
from either a visual or language-based origin.
11
Kalsched (1996) stated that after a traumatic event, memories of the event are
stored in images that form around a complex which later, when stimulated by clusters of
environmental stimuli that resemble the original trauma, is characterized by episodes of
strong affect promulgated by imprints of the original trauma that continue to live
unabated in the psyche (p. 13). He noted that Jung addressed these complexes as
relentless feeling-toned complexes that split off from consciousness and are featured as
demons in dream images (p. 13). Their stock in trade is evidenced in emergent
dissociative states of consciousness that manifest in states of aggression that control and
split the psyche against itself and lead to explosive fits of strong affect that are outside the
will, control, or consciousness of the individual (p. 13). Kalsched observed that this
―splitting is a violent affair—like the splitting of an atom‖ (p. 13). Strong aggression
highlights the presence of primitive defenses in the psyche and thus provides the energy
for disassociation (p. 14). He emphasized that ―outer trauma alone doesn‘t split the
psyche . . . [but that] an inner psychological agencyoccasioned by the traumadoes
the splitting‖ (p. 14).
Kalsched (1996) theorized that when a person experiences trauma, a disabling
affect is metabolized and transmogrified into symbolic processes either in language or
image and then developmentally assimilated into an individual story that continues to live
and shape the victimized psyche (p. 17). He noted that the psyche‘s need to disown
traumatic elements is characterized by unconscious outward aggression at the same time
as traumatic memories are subjectively repressed (p. 17), thus creating a complex that
literally feeds on itself at the same time as it grows in strength and character. Kalsched
observed the basically universal phenomenon that people who have been traumatized
12
cannot mobilize the aggression to disidentify from the negative aspects of experience
because of an internalized sense of bad me caused by trauma (p. 17). He posited that
unconscious repression disallows entry to implicit memories of the event and, in its own
way, serves to protect the psyche. Over time, he said, these inner demons run the lives of
trauma victims taking the form of an inner persecutor with whom the victim identifies
and who interferes with normal development processes for the affected individual (p. 25).
In Kalsched‘s (1996) conception of the dynamic of traumatic reaction, imprints
characterized by negative psychic force fields are managed by an inner saboteur who
surfaces during misguided attempts to achieve homeostasis. As imprints of trauma
persist, functional integration and normal development is divided in a psyche split by a
persecutor complex (p. 26) that manifests in self-destructive behaviors, rage, and
resistance to a reality-driven existence. Kalsched (1998) referred to this persecutor
complex as a guardian being that works as a psychically driven immune defense, which
does not positively serve the life of the psyche or its potential growth but instead turns
against it in a misguided effort to prevent retraumatization (p. 6). He observed that the
trauma survivor therefore exists in a compromised state wherein the vulnerable psyche is
at war with itself and outwardly attacks the parts of experience it perceives as alien in an
effort to disown experiences that nevertheless bind the individual in a complex derived
from the original trauma (1996, p. 24). Healthy object relations are thus negated, said
Kalsched (1996), and fantasy experience is erected to protect the wounded psyche from
further assault (p. 24).
13
Posttraumatic Stress Disorder
PTSD is an anxiety disorder that is brought on by the experience of trauma or by
the witnessing of traumatic events (Nelson, 2013, p. 172). Symptoms associated with
PTSD include ―intrusive memories, emotional avoidance, and heightened physiological
arousal following exposure to a traumatic event‖ (Lenz, Bruijn, Serman, & Bailey, 2014,
p. 360). Secondary symptoms include ―guilt, decreased quality of life, impairment of
mental health, degree of social functioning, and health perceptions‖ (Galovski, Blain,
Mott, Elwood, & Houle, 2012). The epidemiology of trauma that leads to PTSD is
narrowly defined as exposure to combat, assaultive violence including sexual trauma, and
witnessing someone being injured or killed (Perrin et al., 2014, p. 447). According to
conclusions drawn from surveys of random samples in urban areas of eight countries,
20% to 90% of the general population will be exposed to extreme traumatic stressors at
least once in their lifetime (Perrin et al., 2014, p. 449) although only 6.8% to 7.2% will
result in symptomology for PTSD (Sannibale et al., 2013, p. 1397), with women
statistically showing the highest occurrence (Perrin et al., 2014, p. 454). Symptoms of
PTSD have been found to result from a purported inability to integrate traumatizing
events and the trauma remaining is a disassociated form of the event in the effected
individual (Herkt, Tumani, Grön, Kammer, Hofmann & Abler, 2014, p. 2). Although
many people are exposed to extreme stress and develop intrusive symptoms, not all will
develop the panoply of symptoms characteristic to PTSD (van der Volk, 2001, p. 50). In
these other cases, the usual symptomology is incomplete, with only vague memories
reminiscent of the trauma combined with daytime flashbacks, panic attacks, and
nightmares that cannot be voluntarily controlled (Herkt et al., 2014, p. 2). Bessel van der
14
Kolk (2001), a psychiatrist whose research focuses on posttraumatic stress, theorized that
the process of kindling, that is, the persistence of intrusive and repetitious thoughts, sets
up a chronically disordered pattern of arousal that contributes to maintenance of the
syndrome in an individual (p. 50). To compensate for vigilance-associated hyperarousal,
the traumatized individual begins to set up patterns of avoidance to block reminders of
the trauma and eventually becomes numb to both everyday and trauma-related experience
(p. 50). Numbing extends to all aspects of the individual‘s environment and is punctuated
by agitated responses to emotional triggers (p. 50). These reactions, said van der Kolk,
are probably part of a generalized pattern whereby people with PTSD, who are unable to
detect and neutralize qualitative differences between stimuli, necessarily shut down to
their environment in order to compensate for perceptual overwhelm (p. 51).
Trauma‘s neurobiological impact depends on the individual‘s developmental
stage at the time of the trauma (Vosmer, 2012, p. 499) and is often associated with
delayed development (Amir, Brown, & Amit, 1980; Vosmer, 2012, p. 499) and
attachment issues (Arikan & Karanci, 2012, pp. 209-211; Vosmer, 2012). Symptoms of
PTSD in children include hyperarousal, reexperiencing through traumatic memories, and
avoidance (Vosmer, 2012, p. 499). Onset for traumatized children often begins around
the age of 12 and a half years (Biederman et al., 2013, p. 83) and the younger the age of
the onset of the trauma, the more likely is the possibility of PTSD (van der Kolk, Roth,
Pelcovitz, Sunday, & Spinazzola, 2005, p. 394). Other symptoms for PTSD include
emotional dysregulation (van der Kolk, 2001, p. 54), anxiety disorders, hyperarousal, and
behavior disturbances (Vosmer, 2012, p. 500) along with associated comorbidity of
attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) that leads to psychiatric and
15
psychosocial dysfunction (Biederman et al., 2013). Later psychological sequelae include
diagnoses such as alcoholism, social phobia, anxiety disorders, agoraphobia, panic
disorder, and separation anxiety disorder (Biederman et al., 2013, p. 83) as well as
character disorders (van der Kolk et al., 2005, p. 390), bipolar disorder (Spinazzola,
Blaustein, & van der Kolk, 2005, p. 426), and problems in intimacy and relationships
(Zurbriggen, Gobin, & Kaehler, 2012).
Significant variance in PTSD is evident between early trauma victims and later
onset, with greater symptomatology showing in early trauma as well as in cases
characterized by prolonged traumatic events (van der Kolk et al., 2005, p. 394).
Prolonged traumatic events or repeated episodes of trauma can lead to complex PTSD (C-
PTSD) (Herman, 1997), which is similar to but distinct from PTSD. C-PTSD includes
symptomatology relating to emotion dysregulation, self-perceptions, distorted
relationships, and somatization (Roth, Newman, Pelcovitz, van der Kolk, & Mandel,
1997; Spitzer, Chevalier, Gillner, Freyberger, & Barnow, 2006; Taylor, Asmundson, &
Carleton, 2006; van der Kolk et al., 2005).
PTSD’s co-occurrence with other disorders. Although PTSD has been
extensively studied over the past 2 decades, its etiology as the source of other disorders
has been empirically neglected despite trauma‘s obvious insinuation in their formation
(Spinazzola et al., 2005, pp. 426-427). Because PTSD is usually considered separately
from other comorbid conditions, it is delimited in the possibility of its due credit in their
formation because empirical studies usually focus on PTSD symptom criteria only
(Herman, 1992, p. 93; Spinazzola et al., 2005). Clinicians are understandably cautious
when approaching a sensitive issue such as sexual or physical abuse and may tiptoe
16
around it in an effort to maintain therapeutic boundaries and respect for a client‘s
willingness or sensory ability to approach difficult subjects. Nonetheless, when the
etymology of an individual‘s particular psychopathology can be sourced at an original
trauma, it makes sense to unravel the pathology beginning at the point of the trauma,
while taking into account other negative environmental and personality factors that have
subsequently contributed to the individual‘s symptom profile. On this point, van der Kolk
et al. (2005) observed that
psychiatric problems that do not fall within the framework of PTSD are generally
referred to as ―comorbid conditions‖ as if they occurred independently from the
PTSD symptoms. By relegating them to seemingly unrelated ―comorbid‖
conditions, fundamental trauma-related disturbances may be lost to scientific
investigation, and clinicians run the risk of applying treatment approaches that are
not helpful. (p. 396)
Regardless of the discrepancy between the identification of trauma symptoms and
accurate attributions of their etiology, research studies historically place PTSD and C-
PTSD in separate categories from personality disorders despite obvious relationships
between original trauma and the related symptom clusters. To disregard the
symptomology of trauma and its consequent effect on the life and personality of trauma
survivors can result in partial diagnoses and misdirected treatments of the people who
turn to the medical community for help.
Routine comorbidities that co-occur with PTSD present at rates of 80% among
cases most commonly presenting for major depressive disorder or alcohol abuse
(Sannibale et al., 2013; van der Kolk et al., 2005, p. 390). These comorbid disorders are
often treated independently of PTSD, in contrast to treatments for PTSD that focus
exclusively on the imprint of the traumatic memory and exclude treatment for the co-
occuring disorder (van der Kolk et al., 2005, p. 396). Unfortunately, noninclusive
17
treatment regimes that narrow the field to PTSD symptoms neglect other trauma-related
problems that masquerade in the disturbances of other disorders and consequently do not
address the comorbid conditions frequently seen in clinical practice (Spinazzola et al.,
2005, pp. 426-427, 434). Inclusive treatment regimes will generally first approach the
functional impairments caused by the trauma that interfere with daily life before moving
into incorporating treatments that specifically address trauma-related exposure (van der
Kolk et al., 2005, p. 396). More research is called for in terms of advancing innovative
treatments that take into account the complex symptom profiles that often co-occur with
PTSD and that may not have occurred but for the presence of PTSD (Spinazzola et al.,
2005, pp. 426-427).
The neural basis of PTSD and processing extreme stress. According to
research reported by van der Kolk (2001), in response to negative stress events, the
human body organizes multisystemic responses on hormonal and physiological levels.
Following exposure to extreme stress, endogenous, stress-responsive neurohormones are
released in order to enable the organism to respond effectively to stimuli in ways that
increase glucose release or enhance immune function (p. 52). The amygdala and the
hippocampus, integral parts of the limbic function, are also critically affected by stress.
The amygdala integrates internal representations of experience by assigning meaning to
memory images that reflect emotional experience as well as guides behavior that relates
to the associated meaning of the experience for the organism (p. 53). Adjacent to the
amygdala, the hippocampus records and categorizes in memory the spatial and temporal
aspects of the experience via short-term memory, whereby it either moves the memory
into permanent storage or forgets it (p. 54). The hippocampus then sorts the information
18
spatially with previously stored related information and categorizes it according to
rewards (p. 54). Symptoms of PTSD are attributed to hippocampal atrophy and are
evidenced by symptoms such as behavioral disinhibition and environmental
hyperresponsiveness (p. 56). Reductions in hippocampal volume have also been
associated with difficulty in distinguishing between negative and novel conditions and
thus facilitate reexperiencing symptoms (Levy-Gigi, Szabo, Richter-Levin, & Kéri, 2014,
p. 151).
In van der Kolk‘s (2001) review of various studies, he reported that after
narratives relating to personal traumatic events are read back to subjects with PTSD,
increased activation of the amygdala was measured along with parallel activity in the
right visual cortex as they visually reexperience their reported traumatic event (p. 55).
This reexperiencing of trauma in the visual cortex is particularly relevant to the reliving
of traumatic memories in terms of images that later reemerge in behavior characterized
by seemingly irrational nonverbal emotional events. Van der Kolk supported this
hypothesis in his inference that these patients ―experience emotions as physical states
rather than as verbally encoded experiences‖ (p. 55) and pointed to the conclusion that in
PTSD, memories are stored as nonverbal physical states encoded by images. In support
of this conclusion, at the same time as the visual cortex is activated during trauma recall,
the part of the brain that produces speech shuts off (p. 55), reducing the left hemispheric
capacity for rationalization necessary for cognitive operations (van der Kolk & Ducey,
1989, p. 272). Van der Kolk (2001) noted that
Broca‘s area, the part of the left hemisphere responsible for translating personal
experiences into communicable language, . . . [has] a significant decrease in
oxygen utilization during exposure to traumatic reminders. . . . The brain is
19
―having‖ its experience: the person may feel, see, or hear the sensory elements of
the traumatic experience. (p. 55)
This neurological shutdown in Broca‘s area underscores the necessity of emotional and
cognitive processing that comprises cross-hemispheric integration in healing trauma.
PTSD is characterized by ―extreme reactivity to the environment without
intervening reflection‖ (van der Kolk & Ducey, 1989, p. 272). High levels of amygdala
stimulation reduce hippocampal volume and presuppose the misconstruing of threat
responses that result in exaggerated fight or flight responses (van der Kolk, 2001, p. 56).
Van der Kolk (2001) noted a correlation between the activation of a traumatic memory a
person suffers during trauma recall and marked asymmetry in right hemispheric
activation that explains a triggered, nonintegrated experience. The right hemisphere
modulates expression of global nonverbal emotional communication and allows for
assimilation and holistic integration across sensory modalities (p. 56). The left
hemisphere is problem solving: it categorizes perceptual meanings into novel images and
then manipulates words and symbols to express them into culturally derived sets of
meaning (p. 56). According to van der Kolk, vigilant states of emotional arousal are
attributable to deactivation of left hemispheric activity and explain subsequent
exaggerated responses promulgated by antecedent traumatic events that have affected the
right hemisphere‘s role in perception and processing emotional expression (p. 55). He
observed, ―A relative decrease in left hemispheric representation provides an explanation
of why traumatic memories are experienced as timeless and ego-alien; the part of the
brain necessary for generating sequences and for cognitive categorization of experience is
not functioning properly‖ (p. 57). In summary, physiologically, and under stress, people
with PTSD later reenact scenes derivative of early trauma: they see, feel, and hear
20
experience but because of faulty, stress-based neurophysiological processing of
memories, they are detached from subjective experience and without the neural capacity
to negotiate reality functionally with reality-based responses.
Image processing in PTSD. Approaching trauma from other perspectives,
clinical psychologists Mary Long and Randall Quevillon (2009) discussed three
underlying information-processing theories that explain intrusive imagery in PTSD and
are inherent assumptions in most exposure therapies (p. 69; Witvliet, 1997). One of these
theories, developed by research psychologist Peter Lang (1977), proposes a
bioinformational model that suggests that traumatic or fear-induced images are encoded
in neural memory structures that activate cognitive schemas and produce negative affect
(Long & Quevillon, 2009, p. 70). Long and Quevillon (2009) stated that according to
Lang‘s model, to reprocess the image, the client engages ―affectively and physiologically
in the traumatic image through a vivid, detailed description of the image experience‖
(p. 70). The authors reported on a second theory (Foa & Kozak, 1986) that extends
Lang‘s theory of information processing and puts more weight on meaningful internal
structures related to the trauma that must be activated through prolonged imaginal
exposure in order to replace negative imagery with corrective information (Long &
Quevillon, 2009, p. 70). Extending this theory is yet a third (Chemtob, Roitblat, Hamada,
Carlson, & Twentyman, 1988) which asserts that memories are organized hierarchically
with a bias to attend to threat (Long & Quevillon, 2009, p. 70). In this third model, said
Long and Quevillon (2009), ongoing triggers stimulate further activation of PTSD-related
images, affect, and behavior that is hierarchically predisposed to react to threat and to
result in hypervigilance and ―misinterpretation of ambiguous cues‖ (p. 70). Similar to the
21
other scenarios related above, the neural network must be accessed to enable the
occurrence of corrective processes such as habituation and integration of positive
schemas (p. 70). Among exposure therapies, these examples exemplify prototypical
approaches to engaging images in the memory process that comprise the current standard
of practice for treating trauma.
Exposure-Based Interventions for PTSD
Eye movement desensitization and reprocessing (EMDR). EMDR is the most
common treatment modality for PTSD. Therapeutically, an EMDR session engages the
client with titrated exposure to traumatic material at the same time as visual, auditory,
and sensory information relating to the original trauma is processed by the client (Herkt
et al., 2014, p. 1). EMDR targets trauma by desensitizing anxiety and reprocessing
negative schemata through a modality called adaptive information reprocessing (Chen,
Hung, Tsai, Chu, & Chung, 2014, p. 1). Through the use of cross-hemispheric eye
movements, EMDR accesses information processing centers in the brain in order to
create novel responses and replace traumatic memories with positive emotional and
cognitive schemas (pp. 1-2). EMDR is purported to address pathology and personality
development and has been proven to result in significant reductions of anxiety,
depression, and subjective distress as well as other symptoms of PTSD (p. 2, p. 15).
Successful treatments are evidenced neurobiologically in decreased activity in limbic
zones with increased activation of ―prefrontal brain regions related to cognitive control
after completion of successful treatments‖ (Herkt et al., 2014, p. 2).
Virtual reality exposure therapy (VRET). VRET is another exposure-based
intervention that is commonly used today to provide treatment for combat-related PTSD
22
in military service personnel and veterans in safe environments (Nelson, 2013,
pp. 171-172). In her review of related studies, research assistant Rebekah Nelson (2013)
found that exposure therapy uses cognitive and behavioral techniques that accustom the
client to anxiety sources using two interventions: in vivo and imaginal exposure (IE).
These modalities may be poorly tolerated because of the distinct settings of combat-
related trauma and the tendency over time to suppress thoughts that trigger PTSD
symptoms (p. 172). A patient may be retraumatized by such treatments because of
remembering traumatic images that had previously been repressed and were inaccessible
to processing outside the trauma-related network.
VRET employs technology-based treatments to mimic exposure to duplicate
sensory-based environments reminiscent of the original trauma (Nelson, 2013,
p. 172). Nelson (2013) noted that these environments may include exploding improvised
explosive devices, the smell of tires burning, bumps in the road, the clap of overhead
helicopter rotors, and anything else necessary to recreate the scene. This treatment has
proven to be more effective for younger clients who are more used to technology than
older veterans who have not grown up in technological environments (p. 176).
Furthermore, treatment is more effective when administered as close in time to the
original trauma as possible, thus older veterans may not receive the benefits of VRET
after they have been out of service because they have spent a longer time suppressing
their symptoms (p. 177). In addition, Nelson found that a negatively affected therapeutic
alliance has shown to be correlated with therapists multitasking in the process of
controlling complex computer software while administering client treatments.
23
Nonetheless, VRET has proven to be generally successful in treating active duty
members as well as for combat-related PTSD in veterans.
According to Herman (1997), in 1947, one of the postwar treatments for trauma
was narcosynthesis, which presaged contemporary exposure therapies. Derived from
treatments used during wartime, its purpose was to cultivate an immediate intervention
on the front lines in order to return the afflicted soldier as quickly as possible to battle
(p. 25). Utilizing sodium amytal, or in other cases hypnosis, the focus was on ―recovery
and cathartic reliving of traumatic memories, with all the attendant emotions of terror,
rage, and grief‖ (p. 25).
Unfortunately, in the realm of exposure therapies, narcosynthesis is not dissimilar
to VRET, as VRET also desensitizes military personnel from traumatic residue so that
they may return to active duty. Arguably, the same is true for the inheritance of most
contemporary exposure therapies: by recounting details of traumatic events, clients are
benumbed by their own stories and the accompanying emotional catharsistouted as
―cured‖—and then sent back out to the battlefield of family, work, and community.
Although exposure therapies may cognitively and emotionally reprocess a traumatic
event, at the same time, this method further impresses neural networks preestablished by
the trauma itself, and because of the reinforcement of these impressions, its use cannot
guarantee lasting recovery or immunity from relapse.
Prolonged exposure therapy (PE). PE is a well-researched exposure-based
protocol that is also therapeutically applicable for the effects of trauma (van Minnen,
Harned et al., 2012, p. 31). Nonetheless, clinicians are reticent to use this treatment
because of the inherent risks related to the consequences of reexposure to previous
24
trauma during the desensitization process (p. 1). In the dialectic between client and
therapist, PE works to reprocess negative cognitions emotionally by activating fear-
extinction mechanisms absent the schismatic fear outcomes of the original trauma (p. 1).
This method necessitates targeting avoidance behaviors relating to the trauma memory to
reprocess memories (Thompson, Luoma, & LeJeune, 2013, p. 135). PE includes the
following: in vivo exposure in safe situations to reminders and other stimuli related to the
traumatic incident; imaginal exposure (IE) to the trauma memory with processing of
thoughts and feelings related to the trauma; breath training; and education about trauma
(Hoffart, Øktedalen, Langkaas, & Wampold, 2013, p. 471; Zalta et al., 2014, p. 173).
Psychologists Brian Thompson, Jason Luoma, and Jenna LeJeune (2013), who practice
evidence-based psychotherapy and conduct research at Portland Psychotherapy Clinic,
explain that in vivo exposure includes ―safe but distress-evoking activities or situations
that are typically avoided‖ (p. 135) and that IE comprises ―repeated recitation in the
present tense of the target memory followed by a debriefing of the exposure‖ (p. 135).
Other research established the efficacy of PE‘s operational focus on emotional processing
theory (EPT) (Zalta et al., 2014, p. 172). EPT attributes PTSD symptoms to cognitive and
behavioral avoidance behaviors that are bound by perceptions of an incompetent self who
is powerless over a world fraught with threat and danger (p. 172). Progress is made
through reprocessing of erroneous perceptions achieved through disconfirming
information via repeated exposure and emotional processing (p. 172). Ultimately,
―through in vivo and imaginal exposure, patients learn that avoided situations are safe and
that they are capable of coping with distressing situations and memories‖ (p. 172).
25
Because IE involves repeated replays of an individual‘s trauma, there is always
the looming possibility of retraumatization that will undermine successful treatment
outcomes. For this reason, much controversy surrounds this treatment protocol, and
therapists are less likely to consider this treatment as an option for their patients because
of the potential of retraumatization and the unpredictability of this factor. Critical to this
approach is a strong therapeutic alliance between client and therapist. Repeated exposure
to past traumatic and negative emotional events during and between treatments
necessitates the safe container of a well-established therapeutic relationship in order for
the client to endure and mitigate the overwhelming stresses that emerge as a result of
reliving traumatic events (Hoffart et al., 2013, pp. 471-472). Through repeated exposure
to experiences directly aimed at emotional neutralization of traumatic memories, IE can
be effective in reducing trauma-related fear; however, it is not proven to be efficient in
reducing shame, guilt, and anger. In fact, there may be a problem in the induction of
reinforced negative affect through this treatment because exposure does not address
shame and guilt (Dalgleish & Power, 2004). Therapists who choose PE and other
exposure therapies must gamble with the possibility that this protocol will not only be
partially effective but also carries the inherent risk of retraumatization, recidivism, and
failure.
Imagery Treatments
All imagery treatments are designed to undermine intrusive imagery that is
intrinsic to trauma residue. Based in early dynamic theory, imagery rescripting (IR) is a
cognitive-behavior alternative to PE that does not create overwhelming memories for the
client (Hoffart et al., 2013, p. 472; Long & Quevillon, 2009, p. 68). Commonly used to
26
treat nightmares, it can also be applied as a PTSD treatment. IR is ―a unique imagery
technique in which a distressing image is modified in some way in psychotherapy to
change associated negative thoughts, feelings, and/or behaviors‖ (Long & Quevillon,
2009, p. 67). Distressing images and outcomes can be reimagined, for example, with
positive schemata that reframe the traumatic experience and empower the victim.
Mechanisms of change have not been empirically determined in imagery treatments
although imaginal exposure is possibly a tacit mechanism that challenges the impact of
trauma images with present experience and catalyzes the mind‘s ability to reframe and
recreate image scenarios positively (Beck, Emery, & Greenberg, 2005; Germain et al.,
2004; Krakow, 2004).
Summary
For depth psychologists, understanding the basis of trauma from multiple
perspectives can deeply inform an approach to treatment that is effective and
nontraumatizing. Understanding traumatic imprints from a depth psychological
perspective, including image and its role in memory and trauma formation, is invaluable
in therapeutic interactions and can be profoundly compassionate as well as efficacious in
practice. Although these treatments are often not utilized because they are currently
considered alternative and lack the research foundation that is accorded exposure
modalities, they are likely the promising future of trauma therapies. The next section
discusses the efficacy of exposure therapies and proposes alternative holistic and
compassionate treatment paradigms with consideration given to neuroplasticity and the
neurobiology of trauma. While depth psychotherapy compassionately embraces image
and how it plays out it the psyche, a state-of-the-art evidence base regarding the
27
neurophysiology of trauma encourages mental health care practitioners to rethink
exposure therapy in terms of the promise and availability of alternative treatments.
Chapter III
Image, Neuroplasticity, and Alternative Therapies
The Role of Image in the Healing Process
In a way, as one perceives it in the mind, image is like a mirage. In order to
picture something, an image is drawn to mind and a network of associations that relate to
that image arise that give that image context and meaning. Then, the image will recede as
the mind becomes absorbed in its associations. Like a mirage, the image is there, and then
it is not. In the process of deconstructing image in the context of PTSD, image thus
functions one way in the creation of trauma and in another way in the healing process.
From the perspective of image, PTSD is a disorder of memory processing. As
PTSD is characterized by the traumatic effect of images and how they live out in the
psyche, its resolution is well-founded in a corrective process that approaches memory in
such as a way to reframe image positively. Joseph Wolpe (1958), the developer of
systematic desensitization therapy, noted that images were ―specific neural events‖
sequenced by external stimuli (p. 139). In accordance with the premise of exposure
therapies, Wolpe went on to explain that imagery can stand in as the stimulus itself,
bearing the same effect in the mind as the imagined object (p. 39). Unfortunately,
Wolpe‘s generation of theoreticians had to live with a naïveté regarding neurology that
has now been informed by contemporary research methods that enlighten the inquisitor to
specific neural events and processes of memory formation. An ethical necessity demands
incorporation of these advances into treatment and, in this context, because of technical
29
insights into precise neurophenomena, exposure practices are being outdistanced by
contemporary research. Nonetheless, a client who is suffering from PTSD will be less
interested in the amygdala then he will be in relief. Without neglecting the importance
and influence of neuroscientific advances, as a clinician, paying heed to Kalsched‘s
(1997) analysis of trauma and its demons will deeply inform any client encounter and
provide a prodigious therapeutic map to lead the way out.
A Critique of Exposure Therapies
Today, because of its extensive evidence base, exposure therapy is empirically
considered the treatment of choice for PTSD, especially prolonged exposure therapy (PE)
(van Minnen, Harned, et al., 2012; Wells et al., 2014) From an image-processing
perspective, exposure therapies approach healing by revisiting and articulating the image
series compounded within traumatic memories. Through repeated reengagement with
specific details of the trauma that recall image sequences, the individual is enjoined by
the therapist to reprocess these memories emotionally and cognitively. Technically, PE
reengages neural flight, flight, or freeze systems in repeated exposures to the traumatic
event. Desensitization strategies inherent to PE optimally create familiarity with trauma-
related triggers, also known as hot spots, that eventually promulgate nonreactive
responses to the images associated with the memory (Grey, Holmes, & Brewin, 2001,
p. 367). Hot spots are temporal events or triggers characterized by a heightened
sensitivity to specific images inherent to parts of the trauma sequence (p. 367). The
presence and treatment of hot spots comply with the technical diagnosis of PTSD and
require emotional reactions related to the traumatic event that are comprised of fear,
helplessness, and horror (APA, 2013, p. 274). Hot spots are associated with flashbacks,
30
nightmares, and other posttraumatic sequelae and typically cause ―high levels of
emotional distress, may be difficult to recall deliberately to mind, and are associated with
intense reliving of the trauma‖ (Grey et al., 2001, p. 367). In essence and in practice, PE
aggressively shuts down the neural network associated with these traumatic memories
and ameliorates related behaviors through a dramatic process that promotes cognitive and
emotional integration of traumatic residue.
In my estimation, the weakness in this method is that PE reengages survival
mechanisms related to the original trauma and therefore does not promote novel healing
paradigms that replace traumatic imprints with healthy neural networks. Proponents of
PE will argue that this is exactly what happens during prolonged exposure treatments. As
the individual relives the trauma and repeatedly revisits the traumatic memory,
reprocessing does occur and nonreactive neural networks subsequently do develop.
Going against this reasoning is evidence that, despite the fact that the traumatic network
goes dormant after the exposure therapy, the individual still remains vulnerable to a
future constellation of events that potentially can retrigger their susceptibility to the
former traumatic imprints (van der Kolk, 2000, p. 11). PE confuses rigor with change.
Healing can only be achieved by amelioration and is evidenced by complete remission.
This thesis supports the position that exposure therapies can only be a partially
effective treatment for PTSD because of the inherent limitations inferred by
reengagement and processing of preestablished trauma networks. Full and complete
recovery necessitates targeting the multisystemic effects of trauma with the goal to create
novel networks framed by a future resiliency to stress and a vigorous sense of self. Based
on the fact that many evidence-based studies on PE report questionable percentages of
31
success rates, clinicians who heed this warning are correct in their cautious reticence to
engage a client in a process that, as protocol, repeatedly reenacts profoundly negative life
experiences that have already caused enormous amounts of stress, pain, and suffering.
Furthermore, the clients themselves resist PE because of a disinclination to replay their
trauma scripts (Foa & Kozak, 1986). The habit of clinical detachment avoids addressing
the reality behind exposure therapy‘s retraumatization practices and promulgates the
attitude of results at any price. An intelligent investigator, however, can see the wood for
the trees. Sometimes it is quite obvious: for example, after discussing the effectiveness of
PE, Professor of Counseling and clinical psychologist Bita Ghafoori (2012) warned
psychotherapy trainees away from taking their engagement with this treatment protocol
too lightly:
Given the high levels of distress the client will likely face during the first few
exposure exercises, or even as early on as the trauma interview for some, it is
absolutely crucial to discuss in great length the nature and rationale for PE.
Clients must be warned about not only the high levels of distress they may
experience during a session but also the distress they are likely to feel after the
sessions. It must be explained that this is a normal, and even necessary part of
therapy, and that they should not be alarmed or discouraged, as the high levels of
distress are likely to reduce over time as they continue the program. (p. 77)
As a clinician, I believe that purposefully engaging a client in a treatment protocol that
contextually applies extreme distress is unconscionable and as pathological as the
condition itself. Controlled studies of exposure therapy show dropout rates between 20.5
and 32% (Hembree et al., 2003; van Minnen, Arntz, & Keijsers, 2002) with rates of
success measured at only 60% for people who respond well in post-treatment outcomes
related to PTSD and depressive symptoms (Foa et al., 1999; Hagenaars, van Minnen, &
Hoogduin, 2010). In contrast, a 2005 study measured nonresponse rates for PTSD
treatments as high as 67% for PE interventions (Bradley et al., as cited in Rapgay et al.,
32
2014, p. 743). Mental health professionals are ostensibly committed to reducing
emotional distress; for example, other exposure interventions point to acceptance and
tolerance of distress as possibly more efficient for desensitization than repeated exposure
episodes to feared stimuli (Craske et al., 2008; Kircanski et al., 2012). A treatment
modality that applies extreme trauma processing to cure extreme trauma disorders does
not make sense, when there are humane options that can achieve the same results with
lasting effects. Some alternatives are suggested by recent neuroscientific discoveries.
Neuroscientific Contributions to the Pathophysiology of PTSD
Neuroscience contributes specific knowledge to the etiology of PTSD as well as
creates vast potential for developing new treatment modalities. Novel psychological
treatments for PTSD can now rest on a foundation of neurophysiological evidence that
explains the deleterious effects of trauma on psychosocial functioning and the related
inability to thrive. Explicit therapeutic delivery systems based in explanatory evidence
and the concepts of neuroplasticity can precisely address neural deficits caused by PTSD
and potentially fast-forward lasting recovery.
The pathophysiology of PTSD manifests in measurable alterations to many
different parts of the brain including the following:
(1) the parietal lobes, which are thought to integrate information between different
cortical association areas (Damasio, 1989); (2) the amygdala, which evaluates
incoming information for emotional significance and which has been shown to be
activated when people are exposed to reminders of their trauma (Rauch et al.,
1996); (3) the hippocampus, which is thought to create a cognitive map that
allows the categorization of experience, and which has been shown to be
decreased in size in a variety of traumatized populations (Bremner et al., 1995);
(4) the corpus callosum, which allows for the transfer of information by both
hemispheres (Joseph, 1988), integrating emotional and cognitive aspects of the
experience and which has been shown to be decreased in size in adults who were
abused as children (Teicher, 1997); (5) the cingulate gyrus, which is thought to
play a role of both an amplifier and a filter, that helps integrate the emotional and
33
cognitive components of the mind (Devinsky et al., 1995); and which is activated
following effective treatment for PTSD (van der Kolk, 1997) and (6) the
prefrontal cortex, which is involved in problem solving, learning, and complex
stimulus discriminations, and which has been shown to be less activated when
people with PTSD are exposed to reminders of their trauma, and to have increased
activation, relative to pretreatment, after people are effectively treated for PTSD.
(van der Kolk, 2001, p. 49)
Effective treatments for trauma optimally target psychophysiological behavioral
responses through specific interventions that stimulate neuroanatomical structures to
integrate the multisystemic cognitive and emotional aspects of memories implicated by
the PTSD-related event. Specifically, PTSD treatment needs to address the neural
network related to emotional dysregulation, with emphasis on overactivation and
enlargement of the right amygdala in conjunction with memory processing issues
associated with decreased volume in the hippocampus. This in turn will have a ripple
effect on the other hormonal and neural networks implicated in trauma formation.
Neuroplasticity and Behavior Change in PTSD
Neuropsychologists Richard Davidson and Bruce McEwen (2012) noted that the
earliest stance on neuroplasticity was that once neural networks are established, they
endure for the individual‘s lifetime (p. 689). More recent findings confirm, however, that
by appealing to certain neural networks, plastic change may be induced and reversals are
possible, depending on the network involved (p. 691). Applied to PTSD, interventions
can be formulated in relation to specific patterns of brain activity that are effective in
targeted efforts toward emotional and behavioral modification (Davidson & Begley,
2012a; Davidson & Begley, 2012b).
Research indicates that malformations between associated neural systems initiated
by trauma have adverse neurophysiological effects that manifest in behavior. Reduced
34
hippocampal volume, for example, is associated with low self-esteem (Davidson &
McEwen, 2012, p. 690; McEwen, 2007; McEwen & Gianaros, 2011) and may be related
to failure to process contextual information (Dickerson & Eichenbaum, 2010; Goosens,
2011; Moustafa et al., 2013; Rudy, Huff, & Matus-Amat, 2004). Research also indicates
that neurogenesis in the hippocampus is probably stimulated by regular physical exercise
which can address and possibly ameliorate this deficit (Davidson & McEwen, 2012,
p. 690).
Issues relating to emotional processing and well-being are positively associated
with the amygdala and its interconnection with the prefrontal cortex (PFC), whereas
abnormalities in this network are implicated in psychopathology (Davidson & McEwen,
2012, p. 692). The amygdala assigns meaning to sensory information by linking internal
meanings with external objects and associates emotional experiences with these
memories (van der Kolk, 2001, p. 55). Specifically in PTSD, an enlarged right amygdala
is responsible for emotional dysregulation (Davidson & McEwen, 2012, p. 692).
Amygdala activation in response to fear-eliciting stimuli is linked to dispositional
pessimism (Desbordes et al., 2012; Fischer Fischer, Tillfors, Furmark, & Fredrikson,
2001). Davidson and McEwen (2012) associated an overactive amygdala with aggressive
behavior alongside deficits in the PFC correlated with impulsivity and poor executive
functions (p. 690). Noninvasive interventions can be applied to address these problems:
for example, loving kindness meditation can ameliorate aggressive behavior (Kearney et
al., 2013) as well as reduce phobic fear responses to reminders of the traumatic event (pp.
426-427). Moreover, metacognitive awareness can monitor impulsivity and poor
35
executive functions (Wells et al., 2014) as well as help insinuate guidelines for healthy
behavioral alternatives.
Metacognitive Therapy: An Alternative to Prolonged Exposure
Broadly speaking, memories are image sequences processed emotionally in the
limbic areas of the right hemisphere and then cognitively processed through the reason
and language parts of the left hemisphere. Many PTSD treatments approach the disorder
with the premise that in order for hemispheric integration to occur, trauma must be
accessed and then reprocessed both emotionally and cognitively for integration and
resolution. An example is the operating principle of EMDR that utilizes cross-
hemispheric integration through aural or visual stimulation.
Metacognitive therapy (MT) (Wells et al., 2014) is an alternative approach to PE
that embraces the same cognitive mechanisms of change that are operative in PE
modalities (Wells et al., 2014, p. 142). As a different approach, metacognition ends the
victim cycle in trauma through the valuable tool of introspection and self-monitoring in
the healing process. In a process based in the client-therapist relationship, MT entrains
the ability to notice trauma-related images through introspection-related awareness skills
and to turn away from them in a nonreactive way using different behaviors.
Metacognition is the ability to think about thinking, that is, to monitor the
thinking process (Wells, 2000). As a metacognitive process, monitoring refers to the
subjective ability to appraise cognition and knowledge (Koriat & Shitzer-Reichert, 2002).
Metacognition and its emphasis on subjective appraisal supports introspection and the
ability to identify maladaptive responses to the environment that evolve out of trauma
and thus creates the possibility of its opposite: the cultivation and creation of healthy
36
alternatives. Trauma survivors can learn to cultivate the subjective skill of introspection
through a self-monitoring process that supports identification and conversion of
maladaptive thoughts and behaviors that often function as habitual responses to stress.
Once identified, the individual can replace negative behaviors with copacetic patterns that
resonate with the individual‘s conception of a positive self-image.
According to Adrian Wells, Deborah Walton, Karina Lovell, and Dawn Proctor
(2014), clinical psychologists specializing in applications of MT, MT conceptualizes
PTSD not as an aberrant behavior but as a rational coping mechanism in a reflexive
adaptation process (RAP) that uses biases in cognition and attention as coping strategies
during environmental threats (p. 133). Once someone exits the posttrauma anxiety cycle,
however, RAP no longer serves its adaptive purposes, cognition returns to threat-free
processing, and the individual is again asymptomatic (p. 134). Wells et al. (2014)
theorized that PTSD develops because of a posttrauma tendency toward a disruptive
mode of thinking called cognitive attentional syndrome (CAS) that ―consists of cognitive
perseveration in the form of worry/rumination and repeatedly going-over memories in
order to find meaning, prevent harm in the future or fill-in gaps‖ (p. 134). CAS also
includes ongoing threat monitoring, that is, vigilant attention to possible future threats as
well as suppression of negative thoughts or feelings (p. 134). CAS rumination and worry
continually block the individual‘s capacity to reestablish homeostasis and lead to the
ongoing incapacities associated with PTSD (p. 134). The change mechanism is thus
accorded in a parallel process that addresses maladaptive cognitions that, in turn, is
reflected in the reduction of maladaptive symptoms (p. 134). Wells et al. commented,
The metacognitive model suggests that treatment should target the CAS (worry,
rumination, threat monitoring) and metacognitive beliefs rather than focusing on
37
the contents of trauma memory or using PE and reliving. This treatment guides
the client to bring the CAS under flexible control so that threat-modes of
processing subside. (p. 134)
Because research outcomes that use cognitive restructuring with and without PE have
equivalent effects, replacing PE with MT serves the goal of cognitively reframing healing
from trauma. Furthermore, post-treatment results comparing PE with MT show a higher
effect for the MT group over PE (p. 142).
Hemispheric Integration, Emotion-Focused Therapy, and Mindfulness Practices
Because hemispheric integration through affective processing is critical to
recovery from the neurophysiological and neuroanatomical imprints of trauma, principles
of mindfulness-based emotion-focused therapy (EFT) derived from Buddhist theories of
mind can help achieve this resolution by promoting the simultaneous processing of
cognitive and emotional mechanisms implicated in PTSD. In his comments on the
Buddhist Abhidharma texts, Chogyam Trungpa Rinpoche (1975), a prominent Tibetan
Buddhist scholar, noted that image is a primary and spontaneous activity of the mind and
not a secondary byproduct of imagination (p. 31). He related that perception arises after
image formation, then concepts, then behaviorincluding emotions (pp. 31-35). From
perception arises our conception of the world that forms the basis of our experiences. In
turn, these perceptions inform our behavior when we interact with the world.
Deconstructing image in this manner and understanding the trajectory that leads to
emotional responses can deeply inform therapeutic interventions in specific PTSD
treatments.
38
Padmasiri de Silva (2014), a psychologist and proponent of mindfulness-based
EFT, moved this analysis of image one step further and analyzed perception in terms of
the components of emotional processing from a Buddhist perspective:
Within the context of Buddhist psychology, an emotion is more complex than
thoughts: an emotion involves a blend of the cognitive (sanna, cita), the affective
(vedana) and volitional (sankhara) facets, while a broader term motivational
would include volitional/intentions and desires (chanda, raga). To this we also
add the physiological dimension of the body. (p. 136)
De Silva extrapolated that emotion is ―an interactive complex or construction‖
(p. 136) that blends ―cognitive, motivational, affective and physiological arousal factors‖
(p.136) that enable the therapist to operationalize four frames of reference: somatic,
affective, imprints, and phenomena (p. 136). According to EFT theorist and practitioner
Sandra Paivio (2013), EFT assumes that emotions include ―a multimodal network of
information (thoughts, feelings, beliefs, desires, bodily experience), and accessing
emotion accesses this network of emotion‖ (p. 341). She explained that trauma
dysregulates integrated processing of information in these networks. Later, experiences
reminiscent of the original trauma trigger the network, and the once-adaptive response to
trauma is compromised.
Drawing on a Buddhist-based paradigm, De Silva (2014) explained that
mindfulness practice combined with EFT focuses on antidotal methods that restrain
negative emotions, abandon negative emotions when they are present, develop positive
emotions in their place, and enable stabilization of such positive states of mind once they
have developed and, thereby, in accordance with traditional Buddhist practice, transform
negative emotions instead of reject them (pp. 138-139). Mindfulness-based EFT and
emotional processing by inference comprise a therapeutic protocol that synthesizes
39
pansystemic processing modalities in the brain and catalyzes an effect across
hemispheres critical to resolving integration issues that are diagnostically endemic to
PTSD. These practices can target avoidance behaviors, cultivate emotional restraint and
awareness, reduce anxiety and hyperarousal, and ameliorate ADHD difficulties with
increased concentration and attention.
Because healing, well-being, and positive self-states can be achieved through
mind training, neural networks implicated in PTSD can also be targeted through
meditation practices. ―Meditation can be conceptualized as a set of regulatory and self
inquiry mental training regimes cultivated for various ends, including the training of
well-being and psychological health‖ (Chambers, Gullone, & Allen, 2009). Furthermore,
functional and neuronal alterations are evidenced in mind states induced through
meditation training (Chambers et al., 2009; Hölzel et al., 2011; Lutz, Slagter, Dunne, et
al., 2008), whereas neuroplastic changes are connected to behavior change during
cognitive and affective tasks (Hölzel et al., 2011; Lutz, Slagter, Rawlings, et al., 2009).
Meditation as a therapeutic tool is supported by a 2012 longitudinal study that found a
reduction in right amygdala activation in response to emotional stimuli in healthy adults
with no prior meditation experience after an 8-week mindful-attention meditation training
(Desbordes et al., 2012).
Breath training is another approach to emotional dysregulation rooted in Buddhist
mindfulness practices. Samatha meditation is a practice-based breath training modality
that, when taught to clients, supports development of self-observation as well as cognitive
and emotional monitoring skills. Samatha is a powerful regulator of multiple systems in
the body and cultivates mindfulness and introspection through simple observation of the
40
in-and-out breath (Wallace, 2010, p. 47). Mindfulness is achieved through continued
focus on the breath while introspection monitors the ongoing experience toward the
desired effect of achieving stability (Wallace, 2010, p. 47; Desbordes et al., 2012, p. 4).
Mindfulness meditation cultivates . . . faster recovery from setbacks by weakening
the chain of associations that keep us obsessing about and even wallowing in a
setback. It strengthens connections between the prefrontal cortex and the
amygdala, promoting an equanimity that will help keep you from spiraling down.
(Davidson & Begley, 2012a, p. 5)
Samatha meditation has been correlated with classical mindfulness (CM), a practice that
parallels attentional load theory (ATL) in cognitive psychology and neuroscience
(Rapgay et al., 2014). ATL promulgates mastery of a central task or object that becomes
so well known that it becomes second nature to the individual and frees attentional
resources to other peripheral contextual information (Lavie et al., as cited in Rapgay et
al., 2014, p. 744). Rather than create distress and emotional pain, as is typical of exposure
therapies, meditation can address PTSD-related symptomology through a therapeutic
setpoint of control and serenity aimed toward symptom conversion. In an atmosphere of
mindfulness and attention, meditation practice can be applied to relieve stressful states,
decrease hyperarousal, reduce avoidance behaviors and numbing, mitigate dissociative
states, and increase attention.
To address further the emotional and behavioral issues related to emotional
dysregulation, retrospective emotional awareness skills based in introspective awareness
can help identification of trauma-related patterns of behavior due to past maladaptive
emotional processing. In this context, trauma survivors can be encouraged to grieve
collateral losses incurred by faulty defense systems and to forgive themselves in the
present, regardless of misconstrued efforts of the past. A randomized, controlled trial
41
(Shear, Frank, Houck, & Reynolds, 2005) found that PTSD resembles complicated grief
and often co-occurs with major depressive disorder. Engaging grief processes to reframe
trauma can thus functionally replace retraumatization strategies and allow a natural
healing process relating to loss to take place over time. Reframing of past experience
related to trauma in terms of grief processing subverts responsibility based in a victim
mentality; turns the process toward building present-centered awareness and self-esteem;
and structures a revised self-concept based on a positive self-image and empowerment.
Effective reprocessing therefore must include a refocusing that logically calls for
engagement of the whole person and holistic recovery paradigms in the recovery process.
The Scientific Logic of Somatic Therapies
Van der Kolk (2002), a contemporary expert on the neurological and
psychological profiles of PTSD, acknowledged the necessity of applying the lessons of
neuroscience to PTSD and proposed a rationale for incorporating somatic manifestations
of trauma into inclusive treatment paradigms. He stated that in order ―to overcome
traumatic experiences, people require physical experiences that directly contradict the
helplessness and the inevitability of defeat associated with the trauma‖ (p. 383).
Van der Kolk (2002) noted that if the problem for the trauma sufferer is
disassociation, then the process of association should be the goal of treatment (p. 383). In
the sense of linking and evolving images, association thus infers a harmonious healing
process occurring within the individual, a state wherein image associations
commensurate with past experience no longer traumatize but instead are converted in a
healing context toward the goal of an evolved self-image and empowerment. This process
connotes the importance of promoting awareness rather than avoidance of one‘s internal
42
states in ongoing treatment for PTSD. Traditional therapies generally disregard the
sensate qualities associated with the sensory dimension of life despite their obvious
presence and negative influence in many cases. ―This neglect ignores the fact that the
origin of one‘s emotional state is the state of the body‘s chemical profile, the state of
one‘s viscera, and the contraction of the striated muscles of the face, throat, trunk, and
limbs‖ (Damasio, as cited in van der Kolk, 2002, p. 389). As neuroscience research
indicates and because the nature of trauma, on its face, is received by the body as a
somatic shock, it is essential for PTSD treatment-focused paradigms to expand into
somatic therapeutics in combination with other forms of therapy.
In tracing the biochemical substrate of emotions, Candace B. Pert (1986), former
Chief of Brain Chemistry in the clinical neuroscience branch of the National Institute of
Mental Health and a pioneer in mind-body medicine, completed studies that identify the
chemical substrates that putatively explain the interdependence in mind-body
communications. Her research located two major elements in the body‘s ―information
system‖ (p. 8): neuropeptides and the receptors into which they fit. Neuropeptides are
strings of the 16 amino acids that are released directly by DNA, and their sequences
comprise part of the body‘s peptide system (p. 13). Pert was able to define a subtle yet
indelible conversation between the mind and body through the interaction of
neuropeptides and their corresponding receptor sites in both the mind and bodyso
much so that she coined the term bodymind, acknowledging their complex interactions by
integrating the term in discussions of her research. For Pert, the bodies in these
experiments were manifestations of the mind, not the other way around (p. 13). She
noted,
43
The striking pattern of neuropeptide receptor distribution in mood-regulating
areas of the brain, as well as their role in mediating communication throughout
the whole organism, makes neuropeptides the obvious candidates for the
biochemical mediation of emotion. It may be too that each neuropeptide biases
information processing uniquely when occupying receptors at nodal points with
the brain and body. If so, then each neuropeptide may evoke a unique ―tone‖ that
is equivalent to a mood state. (p. 12)
Pert‘s research evidences a highly relevant and evolved communication between the
mind and body that needs to be considered and incorporated into PTSD treatments.
In a therapeutic analysis of somatic treatment Christine Caldwell (1997), somatic
therapist and founder of the of Somatic Psychology department at Naropa University,
noted that thoughts are not just mental events but reflect in the body as well (p. 7).
Caldwell explained that the body/mind is an inseparable feedback loop or continuum and
that any dysfunction within the incorporated organismic continuum will affect the entire
system (p. 7). The communication between the body and mind is thus a critical
correspondence that must be acknowledged in any therapeutic encounter. An original
therapeutic approach that incorporates both contemporary neuroscientific discoveries
discussed in this thesis as well as interpolates the importance of the somatic element in
the healing of trauma is outlined below.
Deep Saltwater Immersion Therapy (DSIT)
Whether the vessel is a sink, a bucket, or a tub, the glee and wonder of water will
always be the same for the small child. Turn on the sprinklers and watch them dance in
the spray! Leave a child in the sand at the shore with a shovel and a bucket, and hours
pass, unfettered by boredom or complaint. Standing at the water‘s edge, one is drawn in,
mesmerized, becalmed, and assuaged by its particular mystery and depth. All who have
experienced the ocean know this and need no research to believe in its settling effect.
44
Nonetheless, there is evidence that confirms the beneficial effect on the human mind and
spirit provided by natural environments, especially those that include water elements.
We are beginning to learn that our brains are hardwired to react positively to
water and that being near it can calm and connect us, increase innovation and
insight, and even heal what‘s broken. Healthy water is crucial to our physiological
and psychological well-being, as well as our ecology and economy. We have a
‗blue mind‘—and it‘s perfectly tailored to make us happy in all sorts of ways that
go far beyond relaxing in the surf, or floating quietly in a pool‖ (Cousteau, 2014,
p. x)
Throughout our lifetimes, experiential factors will always shape our brains, behavior, and
choices. The therapy I propose, DSIT, incorporates an understanding that positive
structural changes in the brain and modifications in behavior can be achieved through
compassionate approaches to therapy and specific training modalities that foster recovery
from trauma and enhance well-being. Furthermore, DSIT turns the corner from other
therapeutic treatments by including somatic healing in natural environments based in
saltwater immersion. As modern innovations related to technology increasingly distance
humankind from the environment and the beneficial effect of natural environments, it has
become essential not to disregard the effect of this loss on the human psyche. It is
therefore common sense to incorporate naturalistic therapeutic encounters into somatic
healing paradigms. As social psychologists Leaf Van Boven and Thomas Gilovich (2003)
observed, ―we live in a world of unprecedented abundance‖ (p. 1193). DSIT‘s
multifaceted approach to trauma treatment is a modern cornucopia that acknowledges this
abundance in the context of the necessity of a holistic approach to trauma treatment.
DSIT is an incorporative approach to trauma based on research in affective
neuroscience and psychological analysis of trauma formation. A true allegiance to
recovery from posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) necessitates consideration of
45
cognitive, affective, and somatic treatments that avoid the retraumatization philosophies
and cathartic approaches to trauma typical of the older desensitization practices. DSIT, by
comparison, engages a naturalistic approach that supports therapeutic and nontraumatic
grieving cycles to process negative memories and other losses incurred while suffering
from this disorder.
Based in a seven-premise multimodal recovery paradigm that incorporates an
innovative somatic recovery component, DSIT applies the latest research in neuroscience
in a holistic positive-psychology format to treat PTSD. These seven premises are the
following:
1. Neuroscientific research on the pathophysiology of PTSD
2. Application of current evidence-based findings in neuroplasticity to remodel
neural networks
3. Metacognitive principles of information processing that foster self-reflection
and positive change
4. Mindfulness-based emotion-focused therapy that promotes hemispheric
integration critical to neurophysiological healing
5. Journaling with DSIT counselors to target hot spots and other persistent
traumatic residue
6. Saltwater immersion therapy
7. Recognition and development of affective style to replace negative processing
and reactivity to trauma triggers.
DSIT is especially unique in its approach to trauma treatment because it incorporates the
element of somatic recovery through immersion in saltwater. The rationale for DSIT
46
holds that this immersion aspect of treatment is both critically therapeutic to a
comprehensive approach to trauma as well as essential to consolidating DSIT‘s
multifaceted approach to recovery.
Chapter IV
Summary and Conclusions
Summary
Due to technical advances in neuroscientific research relating to PTSD, deficits
implicit in this condition can be targeted with appropriate therapeutic modalities aimed
toward ameliorating specific symptoms and neural networks. Exposure therapies have
traditionally been the treatment of choice. These modalities engage cognitive
reprocessing of memories associated with traumatic image sequences in a desensitization
procedure that shuts down emotional symptomology and reactivity through repeated
recountings of the traumatic event. Although desensitization may provide some
remission, it does not provide immunity to the multisystemic imprints of PTSD that could
ultimately induce a holistic and inclusive recovery. Furthermore, clients and clinicians
alike are wary of the retraumatization issues related to this procedure. Deep recovery
must include an incorporative approach to a condition originally precipitated by a global
shock to the mindbody continuum; therefore, effective treatment logically must embrace
a global paradigm of mindbody healing. Because exposure therapies target only
emotional residue and potentially retraumatize clients, they cannot accomplish this
inclusive effect.
Given the challenge of treating the multisystemic manifestation of PTSD, this
thesis proposes that clinicians should first choose to greet all trauma-based client
narratives with a compassionate stance that is advocated by depth approaches to
48
traumatic sequelae. The work of all PTSD modalities should be to meet and welcome all
of the images that comprise trauma, and then turn the traumatic event upside down into a
healing and growth opportunity that creates self-empowerment through successful
survival.
With the advent of new technology and the availability of knowledge relating to
the neurological imprinting of trauma, clinicians now have the option to choose from a
variety of therapeutic modalities that, in each instance, will treat particular aspects of a
client‘s symptomology. Mindfulness-based therapies such as the metacognitive model,
emotion-focused therapies, and breath training such as samatha meditation techniques as
well as the incorporation of somatic and ecopsychological approaches such as saltwater
immersion are all effective, long-term solutions that can create lasting recovery from
trauma and its adverse consequences in the life and well-being of the trauma victim.
Conclusions
Treatments for PTSD intuitively engage image in efforts to ameliorate the effects
of trauma. It is worth noting that it is the continued presence of older images that
perpetuates the imprints of trauma in the psyche and, furthermore, it is a process of
revisioning images into the future that promises to lead the way out. Although exposure
therapies attempt to affect this solution and, perhaps, for a while were the best option to
do so, in light of contemporary neuroscientific research, this is no longer true.
Contemporary neuroimaging technology and findings that pinpoint the neurobiology of
trauma comprise a metaphoric lighthouse that brings the vessel of curative paradigms to
safe harbor with the promise of innovative cures. Cognitive and emotional processing can
occur without reinforcing the neural and physiological networks embattled by imprints of
49
trauma that instilled the disorder in the first place. Alternative therapies that teach
metacognition, encourage insightful affective processing, and promote introspective skills
combined with mindfulness practices support remission and cure in the future without the
retraumatizing paradigms that we have inherited from the treatment of soldiers affected in
the wars of the 20th century.
Implications of the research for psychotherapy. Contemporary research in
neuroscience has outpaced the usefulness of exposure therapies in the treatment of PTSD.
On the one hand, an explanatory depth psychological approach to the manifestations of
traumatic memory in the psyche parallels the formation of neurophysiological imprints of
trauma and remains an enduring and compassionate stance to address suffering in the
therapeutic container, as exemplified by Kalsched‘s (1996, 1998) work in the field. On
the other hand, mainstream clinical approaches to trauma that rely on exposure modalities
are clearly outdated. It is clear from a review of the extant research that exposure
treatments for PTSD are due for an overhaul. In an atmosphere that maintains a
compassionate stance to disease and the methods used to alleviate suffering, therapists do
not need to tiptoe into trauma narratives and then, once in the garden, trample the daisies.
By revisioning the role of image out of trauma and into recovery paradigms, we can
incorporate healing images into a context that positively supports an individual‘s
survival, recovery, and future self-empowerment. As therapists, we can choose to sustain
a compassionate dialogue that directly confronts the sagas of trauma at the same time as
we provide humane options whereby the client may choose to deal with the trauma.
Recommendations for further research. More research is necessary to
investigate the conversation in the mindbody relationship, particularly in the area of
50
somatic therapies for treatment of PTSD. Although, currently, no research has been
conducted regarding the beneficial effects of saltwater immersion therapy, conceptually,
the proposed DSIT parallels other treatment approaches that embrace ecopsychological
and somatic healing paradigms. Nonetheless, research relating to saltwater immersion in
the future could be informative, especially in relation to its effect on heart rates during
recounting of trauma narratives.
References
American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association (2013) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
disorders (5th ed.). Washington D.C.: Author.
Amir, S., Brown, Z., & Amit, Z. (1980). The role of endorphins in stress: Evidence and
speculations. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 4(1), 77-86. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1016/0149-7634(80)90027-5
Arikan, G., & Karanci, N. (2012). Attachment and coping as facilitators of posttraumatic
growth in Turkish university students experiencing traumatic events. Journal of
Trauma and Dissociation, 13(2), 209-225. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15299732
.2012.642746
Beck, A. T., Emery, G., & Greenberg, R. L. (2005). Anxiety disorders and phobias: A
cognitive perspective. New York, NY: Basic Books.
Biederman, J. J., Petty, C. R., Spencer, T. J., Woodworth, K. Y., Bhide, P. P., Zhu, J. J.,
& Faraone, S. V. (2013). Examining the nature of the comorbidity between
pediatric attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and posttraumatic stress
disorder. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 128(1), 78-87. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1111/acps.12011
Caldwell, C. (1997). Getting in touch: The guide to new body-centered therapies.
Wheaton, IL: Quest Books.
Chambers, R., Gullone, E., & Allen, N. B. (2009). Mindful emotion regulation: An
integrative review. Clinical Psychology Review, 29(6), 560-572. http://dx.doi.org
/10.1016/j.cpr.2009.06.005
Chemtob, C. M., Roitblat, H. L., Hamada, R. S., Carlson, J. G., & Twentyman, C. T.
(1988). A cognitive action theory of posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of
Anxiety Disorders, 2, 253-275. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0887-6185(88)90006-0
Chen, Y. R., Hung, K. W., Tsai, J. C., Chu, H., & Chung, M. H. (2014). Efficacy of eye-
movement desensitization and reprocessing for patients with posttraumatic-stress
disorder: A meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. PLOS ONE, 9(8).
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0103676
Cousteau, C. (2014). Foreword. In W. J. Nichols, Blue mind (pp. ix-xii). New York, NY:
Little.
52
Craske, M. G., Kircanski, K., Zelikowsky, M., Mystkowski, J., Chowdhury, N., & Baker,
A. (2008). Optimizing inhibitory learning during exposure therapy. Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 46(1), 5-27. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2007.10.003
Dalgleish, T., & Power, M. J. (2004). Emotion-specific and emotion-non-specific
components of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD): Implications for a taxonomy
of related psychopathology. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42(9), 1069-1088.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2004.05.001
Damasio, A. R. (1989). Time-locked multiregional retroactivation: A systems-level
proposal for the neural substrates of recall and recognition. Cognition, 33(1-2),
25-62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(89)90005-X
Davidson, R. J., & Begley, S. (2012a). The emotional life of your brain. New York, NY:
Hudson Street Press.
Davidson, R. J., & Begley, S. (2012b). The new science of feelings. Newsweek,
159(9/10), pp. 46-51.
Davidson, R. J., Jackson, D. C., & Kalin, N. H. (2000). Emotion, plasticity, context, and
regulation: Perspectives from affective neuroscience. Psychological Bulletin,
126(6), 890-909. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.126.6.890
Davidson, R. J., & McEwen, B. S. (2012). Social influences on neuroplasticity: Stress
and interventions to promote well-being. Nature Neuroscience, 15(5), 689-695.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn.3093
de Silva, P. (2014). An introduction to Buddhist psychology and counseling: Pathways of
mindfulness-based therapies (5th ed.). New York, NY: Palgrave.
Desbordes, G., Negi, L. T., Pace, T. W., Wallace, B. A., Raison, C. L., & Schwartz, E. L.
(2012, November 1). Effects of mindful-attention and compassion meditation
training on amygdala response to emotional stimuli in an ordinary, non-meditative
state. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 1-15. http://dx.doi.org/10
.3389/fnhum.2012.00292
Dickerson, B. C., & Eichenbaum, H. (2010). The episodic memory system:
Neurocircuitry and disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology, 35(1), 86-104.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/npp.2009.126
Finlay, L. (2012). Unfolding the phenomenological research process: Iterative stages of
―seeing afresh.‖ Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 53(2), 172-201. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1177/0022167812453877
53
Fischer, H., Tillfors, M., Furmark, T., & Fredrikson, M. (2001). Dispositional pessimism
and amygdala activity: A PET study in healthy volunteers. Neuroreport: For
Rapid Communication of Neuroscience Research, 12(8), 1635-1638.
doi:10.1097/00001756-200106130-00024
Foa, E. B., Dancu, C. V., Hembree, E. A., Jaycox, L. H., Meadows, E. A., & Street, G. P.
(1999). A comparison of exposure therapy, stress inoculation training, and their
combination for reducing posttraumatic stress disorder in female assault victims.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 67(2), 194-200. http://dx.doi.org
/10.1037/0022-006X.67.2.194
Foa, E. B., & Kozak, M. J. (1986). Emotional processing of fear: Exposure to corrective
information. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 20-35.
Galovski, T. E., Blain, L. M., Mott, J. M., Elwood, L., & Houle, T. (2012). Manualized
therapy for PTSD: Flexing the structure of cognitive processing therapy. Journal
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 80(6), 968-981. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037
/a0030600
Germain, A., Krakow, B., Faucher, B., Zadra, A., Nielsen, T., Hollifield, M., . . . Koss,
M. (2004). Increased mastery elements associated with imagery rehearsal
treatment for nightmares in sexual assault survivors with PTSD. Dreaming, 14(4),
195-206. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1053-0797.14.4.195
Ghafoori, B., & Davaie, S. (2012). Training student therapists in prolonged exposure
therapy: A case study demonstrating teaching, supervising, and learning a trauma
focused treatment. Traumatology, 18(4), 72-78. doi:10.1177/1534765612438946
Goosens, K. A. (2011). Hippocampal regulation of aversive memories. Current Opinion
in Neurobiology, 21(3), 460-466. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2011.04.003
Grey, N., Holmes, E., & Brewin, C. R. (2001). Peritraumatic emotional ―hot spots‖ in
memory. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 29(3), 367-372. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1017/S1352465801003095
Hagenaars, M. A., van Minnen, A., & Hoogduin, K. A. (2010). The impact of
dissociation and depression on the efficacy of prolonged exposure treatment for
PTSD. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48(1), 19-27. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016
/j.brat.2009.09.001
Hembree, E. A., Foa, E. B., Dorfan, N. M., Street, G. P., Kowalski, J., & Tu, X. (2003).
Do patients drop out prematurely from exposure therapy for PTSD? Journal of
Traumatic Stress, 16(6), 555-562. doi:10.1023/B:JOTS.0000004078.93012.7d
Herkt, D., Tumani, V., Grön, G., Kammer, T., Hofmann, A., & Abler, B. (2014).
Facilitating access to emotions: Neural signature of EMDR stimulation. PLOS
ONE, 9(8), 1-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0106350
54
Herman, J. (1992). Complex PTSD: A syndrome in survivors of prolonged and repeated
trauma. In M. J. Horowitz (Ed.), Essential papers on posttraumatic stress
disorder (pp. 82-98). New York, NY: New York University Press.
Herman, J. (1997). Trauma and recovery: The aftermath of violenceFrom domestic
abuse to political terror. New York, NY: Basic Books.
Hillman, J. (1981). Archetypal psychology: A brief account. Dallas, TX: Spring.
Hoffart, A., Øktedalen, T., Langkaas, T., & Wampold, B. E. (2013). Alliance and
outcome in varying imagery procedures for PTSD: A study of within-person
processes. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 60(4), 471-482. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1037/a0033604
Hölzel, B. K., Carmody, J., Vangel, M., Congleton, C., Yerramsetti, S. M., Gard, T., &
Lazar, S. W. (2011). Mindfulness practice leads to increases in regional brain gray
matter density. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 191(1), 36-43. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2010.08.006
Jung, C. G. (1980). Religion and psychology: A reply to Martin Buber (R .F. C. Hull,
Trans.). In H. Read et al. (Series Eds.), The collected works of C. G. Jung (Vol.
18, pp. 663-670). Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. (Original work
published 1952)
Kalsched, D. (1996). The inner world of trauma: Archetypal defenses of the personal
spirit. New York, NY: Routledge.
Kalsched, D. (1998). Archetypal defenses in the clinical situation: A vignette. Journal of
Analytical Psychology, 43(1), 3-17. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1465-5922.00003
Kearney, D. J., Malte, C. A., McManus, C., Martinez, M. E., Felleman, B., & Simpson,
T. L. (2013). Loving-kindness meditation for posttraumatic stress disorder: A
pilot study. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 26(4), 426-434. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002
/jts.21832
Kircanski, K., Mortazavi, A., Castriotta, N., Baker, A. S., Mystkowski, J. L., Yi, R., &
Craske, M. G. (2012). Challenges to the traditional exposure paradigm:
Variability in exposure therapy for contamination fears. Journal of Behavior
Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 43(2), 745-751. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016
/j.jbtep.2011.10.010
Koriat, A., & Shitzer-Reichert, R. (2002). Metacognitive judgments and their accuracy.
In P. Chambres, M. Izaute, & P. Marescaux (Eds.), Metacognition: Process,
55
function and use (pp. 1-17). Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1007/978-1-4615-1099-4_1
Krakow, B. (2004). Imagery rehearsal therapy for chronic posttraumatic nightmares: A
minds eye view. In R. I. Rosner, W. J. Lyddon, & A. Freeman (Eds.), Cognitive
therapy and dreams (pp. 89-109). New York, NY: Springer.
Lang, P. J. (1977). Imagery in therapy: An information processing analysis of fear. In M.
J. Horowitz (Ed.), Essential papers on posttraumatic stress disorder
(pp. 387-412). New York, NY: New York University Press.
Lenz, S., Bruijn, B., Serman, N. S., & Bailey, L. (2014). Effectiveness of cognitive
processing therapy for treating posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Mental
Health Counseling, 36(4), 360-376.
Levy-Gigi, E., Szabo, C., Richter-Levin, G., & Kéri, S. (2014). Reduced hippocampal
volume is associated with overgeneralization of negative context in individuals
with PTSD. Neuropsychology, 29(1), 151-161. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037
/neu0000131
Long, M. E., & Quevillon, R. (2009). Imagery rescripting in the treatment of
posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 23(1), 67-76.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1891/0889-8391.23.1.67
Lutz, A., Slagter, H. A., Dunne, J. D., & Davidson, R. J. (2008). Attention regulation and
monitoring in meditation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(4), 163-169. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.01.005
Lutz, A., Slagter, H. A., Rawlings, N. B., Francis, A. D., Greischar, L. L., & Davidson,
R. J. (2009). Mental training enhances attentional stability: Neural and behavioral
evidence. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29(42), 13418-13427. http://dx.doi.org
/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1614-09.2009
Mazoyer, B., Tzourio-Mazoyer, N., Mazard, A., Denis, M., & Mellet, E. (2002). Neural
bases of image and language interactions. International Journal of Psychology,
37(4), 204-208. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00207590244000007
McEwen, B. S. (2007). Physiology and neurobiology of stress and adaptation: Central
role of the brain. Physiological Reviews, 87(3), 873-904. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152
/physrev.00041.2006
McEwen, B. S., & Gianaros, P. J. (2011). Stress- and allostasis-induced brain plasticity.
Annual Review of Medicine, 62(1), 431-445. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-
med-052209- 100430
Merleau-Ponty, M. (1968). The visible and the invisible (A. Lingis, Trans.). Evanston, IL:
Northwestern University Press. (Original work published 1964)
56
Moustafa, A. A., Gilbertson, M. W., Orr, S. P., Herzallah, M. M., Servatius, R. J., &
Myers, C. E. (2013). A model of amygdala-hippocampal-prefrontal interaction in
fear conditioning and extinction in animals. Brain and Cognition, 81(1), 29-43.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2012.10.005
Nelson, R. J. (2013). Is virtual reality exposure therapy effective for service members and
veterans experiencing combat-related PTSD? Traumatology, 19(3), 171-178.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1534765612459891
Paivio, S. C. (2013). Essential processes in emotion-focused therapy. Psychotherapy,
50(3), 341-345. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0032810
Perrin, M., Vandeleur, C., Castelao, E., Rothen, S., Glaus, J., Vollenweider, P., & Preisig,
M. (2014). Determinants of the development of post-traumatic stress disorder, in
the general population. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 49(3),
447-457. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00127-013-0762-3
Pert, C. B. (1986). The wisdom of the receptors: Neuropeptides, the emotions and
bodymind. Advances in Mind-Body Medicine, 3(3), 8-16.
Rapgay, L., Ross, J. L., Petersen, O., Izquierdo, C., Harms, M., Hawa, S., . . . Couper, G.
(2014). A proposed protocol integrating classical mindfulness with prolonged
exposure therapy to treat posttraumatic stress disorder. Mindfulness, 5(6),
742-755. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12671-013-0231-9
Roth, S., Newman, E., Pelcovitz, D., van der Kolk, B., & Mandel, F. S. (1997). Complex
PTSD in Victims Exposed to Sexual and Physical Abuse: Results from the DSM-
IV Field Trial for Posttraumatic Stress Disorder. Journal Of Traumatic Stress,
10(4), 539-555.
Rudy, J. W., Huff, N. C., & Matus-Amat, P. (2004). Understanding contextual fear
conditioning: Insights from a two-process model. Neuroscience and
Biobehavioral Reviews, 28(7), 675-685. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev
.2004.09.004
Sannibale, C., Teesson, M., Creamer, M., Sitharthan, T., Bryant, R. A., Sutherland, K., &
Peek-OLeary, M. (2013). Randomized controlled trial of cognitive behaviour
therapy for comorbid post-traumatic stress disorder and alcohol use disorders.
Addiction, 108(8), 1397-1410. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/add.12167
Shalit, E. (2010). Destruction of the image and the worship of transiency. Jung Journal:
Culture and Psyche, 4(1), 85-98. http://dx.doi.org/10.1525/jung.2010.4.1.85
Shear, K., Frank, E., Houck, P. R., & Reynolds, C. I. (2005). Treatment of complicated
grief: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of the American Medical
Association, 293(21), 2601-2608. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jama.293.21.2601
57
Silverstein, S. M. (2014). Jungs views on causes and treatments of schizophrenia in light
of current trends in cognitive neuroscience and psychotherapy research: I.
Aetiology and phenomenology. Journal of Analytical Psychology, 59(1), 98-129.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1468-5922.12057
Spinazzola, J., Blaustein, M., & van der Kolk, B. A. (2005). Posttraumatic stress disorder
treatment outcome research: The study of unrepresentative samples? Journal of
Traumatic Stress, 18(5), 425-436. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jts.20050
Spitzer, C., Chevalier, C., Gillner, M., Freyberger, H. J., & Barnow, S. (2006). Complex
posttraumatic stress disorder and child maltreatment in forensic inpatients.
Journal of Forensic Psychiatry and Psychology, 17(2), 204-216. http://dx.doi.org
/10.1080/14789940500497743
Stein, M. (2008). ‗Divinity expresses the self . . .‘ An investigation. Journal of Analytical
Psychology, 53(3), 305-327. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-5922.2008.00729.x
Taylor, S., Asmundson, G. G., & Carleton, R. (2006). Simple versus complex PTSD: A
cluster analytic investigation. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 20(4), 459-472.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2005.04.003
Thompson, B. L., Luoma, J. B., & LeJeune, J. T. (2013). Using acceptance and
commitment therapy to guide exposure-based interventions for posttraumatic
stress disorder. Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy, 43(3), 133-140.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10879-013-9233-0
Trungpa, C. (1975). Glimpses of Abhidharma. Boston, MA: Shambhala.
Van Boven, L., & Gilovich, T. (2003). To do or to have? That is the question. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 85(6), 1193-1202. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037
/0022-3514.85.6.1193
van der Kolk, B. A. (2000). Posttraumatic stress disorder and the nature of trauma.
Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 2(1), 7-22.
van der Kolk, B. A. (2001). The psychobiology and psychopharmacology of PTSD.
Human Psychopharmacology: Clinical and Experimental, 16(Suppl1), 49-64.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hup.270
van der Kolk, B. A. (2002). Posttraumatic therapy in the age of neuroscience.
Psychoanalytic Dialogues, 12(3), 381-392. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080
/10481881209348674
van der Kolk, B. A., & Ducey, C. P. (1989). The psychological processing of traumatic
experience: Rorschach patterns in PTSD. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 2(3), 259-
274. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jts.2490020303
58
van der Kolk, B. A., Roth, S., Pelcovitz, D., Sunday, S., & Spinazzola, J. (2005).
Disorders of extreme stress: The empirical foundation of a complex adaptation to
trauma. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 18(5), 389-399. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jts
.20047
van Minnen, A., Arntz, A., & Keijsers, G. J. (2002). Prolonged exposure in patients with
chronic PTSD: Predictors of treatment outcome and dropout. Behaviour Research
and Therapy, 40(4), 439-457. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(01)00024-9
van Minnen, A., Harned, M. S., Zoellner, L., & Mills, K. (2012). Examining potential
contraindications for prolonged exposure therapy for PTSD. European Journal of
Psychotraumatology, 3, 1-14. http://dx.doi.org/10.3402/ejpt.v3i0.18805
Vosmer, S. (2012). The usefulness of group analysis in the conceptualization and
treatment of ‗personality disorders‘ and complex/post-traumatic stress disorder.
Group Analysis, 45(4), 498-514. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0533316412462526
Wallace, A. (2010). The four immeasurables: Practices to open the heart. Ithaca, NY:
Snow Lion.
Wells, A. (2000). Emotional disorders and metacognition: Innovative cognitive therapy.
New York, NY: Wiley.
Wells, A., Walton, D., Lovell, K., & Proctor, D. (2014, August 10). Metacognitive
therapy versus prolonged exposure in adults with chronic post-traumatic stress
disorder: A parallel randomized controlled trial. Cognitive Therapy and Research.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10608-014-9636-6
Whitmont, E. (1991) The symbolic quest. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Witvliet, C. (1997). Traumatic intrusive imagery as an emotional memory phenomenon:
A review of research and explanatory information processing theories. Clinical
Psychology Review, 17(5), 509-536. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0272-7358
(97)00025-1
Wolpe, J. (1958). Psychotherapy for reciprocal inhibition. Stanford, CA: Stanford
University Press.
Zalta, A. K., Gillihan, S. J., Fisher, A. J., Mintz, J., McLean, C. P., Yehuda, R., & Foa, E.
B. (2014). Change in negative cognitions associated with PTSD predicts symptom
reduction in prolonged exposure. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
82(1), 171-175. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0034735
Zurbriggen, E. L., Gobin, R. L., & Kaehler, L. A. (2012). Trauma, attachment, and
intimate relationships. Journal of Trauma and Dissociation, 13(2), 127-133.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15299732.2012.642762